ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
|
The Project
directors acknowledge the vital support and contributions of many people
to this report, including:
• the very dedicated staff of the Educational Assessment Research Unit
• Heleen Visser, Hadyn Green and other staff members of the Ministry of Education
• members of the Project’s National Advisory Committee
• members of the Project’s Mathematics Advisory Panel
• principals and children of the schools where tasks were trialled
• principals, staff and Board of Trustee members of the 246 schools included in the 2008 sample
• the 2867 children who participated in the assessments and their parents
• the 96 teachers who administered the assessments to the children
• the 43 senior tertiary students who assisted with the marking process
• the 192 teachers who assisted with the marking of tasks early in 2009.
• the people and organisations who granted permission for the publication of their work in this report,
to illustrate our assessment resources. [see Resource Acknowledgments for full details and rights]
OVERVIEW
|
This
report addresses student knowledge, strategies and skills in
four areas of mathematics: number, measurement, geometry and
statistics. More than half of the assessment tasks were number
tasks. Year 8 students, on average, performed at a substantially
higher level than year 4 students, but on most tasks there
was a substantial overlap in performance. Mathematics is a
popular subject, second in popularity among year 4 subjects
and third among year 8 subjects.
Performance in mathematics did not improve overall between 2005
and 2009, although there were marked differences from this pattern
on some individual tasks. Taking a longer term view, there is evidence
of a small improvement for year 4 students from 1997 to 2009, although
this has been constrained by a drop in performance on tasks requiring
quick recall or derivation of number facts. Over the same 12-year
period there has been no meaningful performance change overall
for year 8 students.
On average, year 4 boys perform a little better than girls, with
no meaningful difference for year 8 boys and girls. At both year
levels, Pakeha students averaged moderately to strongly higher
than Mäori students and strongly higher than Pasifika students,
but there were exceptions on some tasks (for instance, Pasifika
students performed similarly to Pakeha students on most addition
tasks). There always was a substantial overlap in performance,
with students of all ethnicities among the high and low performers
on each task. Over the last 12 years there has been no clear downwards
or upwards trend in performance differences among the ethnic subgroups,
nor in the high proportion of mathematics tasks showing performance
differences by school decile rating. |
The NEMP Approach to National Monitoring |
|
New
Zealand’s National Education Monitoring Project commenced
in 1993, with the task of assessing and reporting on the achievement
of New Zealand primary school children in all areas of the
school curriculum. Children are assessed at two class levels:
year 4 (halfway through primary education) and year 8 (at the
end of primary education). Different curriculum areas and skills
are assessed each year, over a four-year cycle. The main goal
of national monitoring is to provide detailed information about
what children know, think and can do, so that patterns of performance
can be recognised, successes celebrated, and desirable changes
to educational practices and resources identified and implemented.
Each year, random samples of children are selected nationally, then assessed
in their own schools by teachers specially seconded and trained for this work.
Task instructions are given orally by teachers, through video presentations,
on laptop computers, or in writing. Many of the assessment tasks involve the
children in the use of equipment and materials. Their responses are presented
orally, by demonstration, in writing, in computer files, or through submission
of other physical products. Many of the responses are recorded on videotape for
subsequent analysis. |
Assessing
Mathematics |
|
In
2009, the third year of the fourth cycle of national monitoring,
three areas were assessed: mathematics, social studies, and information
skills. This report presents details of the mathematics assessments.
The use of many tasks with both year 4 and year 8 students allows
comparisons of the performance of year 4 and 8 students in 2009.
Because about 45% of the tasks have been used twice, in both 2005
and 2009, trends in performance across that four-year period can
also be analysed. Four tasks allow direct consideration of longer-term
trends: two with data from 1997 and 2009, and two with data from
2001 as well as from 2005 and 2009.
Chapter 2 explains the place of mathematics in the New Zealand
curriculum and presents the mathematics framework. It identifies
four areas of content (number and algebra, measurement, geometry,
and statistics) linked to eight processes. The importance of attitudes
and motivation is also highlighted.
|
|
Number
and Algebra |
|
Chapter 3 presents
the students’ results on 56 number and algebra
tasks. There was strong progress from year 4 to year 8. Averaged
across 217 task components administered to year 4 and year 8
students in 2009, 30% more year 8 than year 4 students succeeded
with these components.
Overall, performance at both year levels was unchanged between
2005 and 2009. Averaged across 137 task components attempted by
year 4 students in both years, the same percentage of students
succeeded in 2009 as in 2005. At year 8 level also, on average,
across 172 task components, the same percentage succeeded in 2009
as in 2005. The most notable change in performance was a decline
for year 8 students on multiplication problems (p36), where changes
in computation strategy were clearly evident.
Three tasks allowed study of trends over periods longer than four
years. One involved number patterns and sequences, with substantial
improvement from 1997 to 2009 for year 4 students and smaller improvement
for year 8 students. Two tasks involving knowledge of addition
and multiplication facts had been used in the 2001 and 2005 assessments,
when they showed substantial losses for year 4 students in both
areas and a small loss for year 8 students on multiplication facts.
There was negligible further change on these tasks between 2005
and 2009.
Students at both levels scored poorly in tasks involving estimation
and tasks involving fractions (especially fractions other than
halves and quarters). There was clear evidence that students have
adopted changes in number strategy taught in recent years. This
appears to have been advantageous in responses to some tasks and
disadvantageous in responses
to other tasks.
|
Measurement |
|
Chapter 4 presents
the results for 25 measurement tasks. There was strong progress
from year 4 to year 8. Averaged across 95 task components administered
to both year 4 and year 8 students, 28% more year 8 than year 4
students succeeded with these components.
Overall, there was no evidence of change between 2005 and 2009 for
year 4 students, but a slight reduction in the performance of year
8 students. Averaged across 34 trend task components attempted by
year 4 students in both years, the same percentage succeeded in 2009
as in 2005. At year 8 level, on average across 59 task components,
2.5% fewer students succeeded in 2009 than in 2005.
A good range of measurement systems, processes and applications was
covered in the set of tasks attempted by students. At both levels
students’ skills of reading measurements were substantially
stronger than those of making good estimations. Year 8 students were
quite weak in the understanding of perimeter, area and volume.
|
Geometry |
|
Chapter
5 presents the results for 13 geometry tasks.
There was quite strong progress from year 4 to year 8. Averaged
across 15 task components administered to both year 4 and year
8 students, 21% more year 8 than year 4 students succeeded
with these components.
Overall, there was no meaningful change in performance for
year 4 or year 8 students between 2005 and 2009. Averaged across
17 trend task components attempted by year 4 students in both
years, 2% more students succeeded in 2009 than in 2005, but
the small number of tasks and components mean that this change
should not be regarded as meaningful. At year 8 level, the
same percentage of students succeeded on 41 task components
in 2009 as in 2005.
A small decline in capability to identify cross sections of
three-dimensional objects was evident in one task previously
used in the 1997 assessments.
Many students were able to identify the symmetry lines of two-dimensional
shapes,
and year 8 students had good success with drawing the nets
of some three-
dimensional objects. Students had less success with visualising
the internal structure and cross sections of three-dimensional
objects, and with following instructions involving angle measurements
expressed in fractions of complete turns or in degrees.
|
Statistics |
|
Chapter
6 presents the results of six statistics tasks.
Readers should note that much of what is usually taught and
assessed in this area is covered in separate NEMP reports on
using Graphs, Tables and Maps: most recently Report 46 on the
2007 assessments. The one task administered at both year 4
and year 8 in 2009 showed moderate growth, with, on average,
a 12% increase in performance from year 4 to year 8 on seven
task components.
Year 4 students improved markedly on one trend task between
2005 and 2009, with little change on the other trend task.
There was no meaningful change between 2005 and 2009 across
three trend tasks for year 8 students.
Students generally performed well on tasks related to recording
or directly interpreting data, but much less well in applying
probability-related ideas to data. |
Survey |
|
Chapter
7 focuses on the results of a survey that sought
information from students about their strategies for, involvement
in, and enjoyment of mathematics. Mathematics was the second most
popular of 14 subjects for year 4 students and the third most popular
for year 8 students, the same result as in 2005 and one place higher
at both levels than in 2001.
An open-ended question asked students, “What are some interesting
maths things you do in your own time?” The emphasis on basic
facts and tables among year 4 students had declined substantially
between 2001 and 2005, from 56% to 36% of students, but increased
in 2009 to 47% of year 4 students.
The student responses to 11 rating items showed that about 10%
more year 8 than year 4 students have distinctly negative views
about studying mathematics in school and about their own capabilities,
while 32% more year 8 than year 4 students are negative about doing
maths in their own time. These patterns have stayed quite consistent
from the first survey in 1997 to the 2005 survey. Over the same
period, there have been moderate reductions
in the percentages of students who said that they didn’t
know how good their parents thought they were at maths, or how
good their teacher thought that they were at maths.
|
Performance
of Subgroups |
|
Chapter
8 details the results of analyses comparing
the performance of different demographic subgroups. Community size,
school size, school type (for year 8 students) and geographic zone
did not seem to be important factors predicting achievement on
the mathematics tasks. The same was true for the 2005, 2001 and
1997 assessments. However, there were statistically significant
differences in
the performance of students from low, medium and high decile schools
on 85% of
the tasks at year 4 level (compared to 63% in 2005, 87% in 2001
and 85% in 1997) and 83% of the tasks at year 8 level (compared
to 65% in 2005, 76% in 2001 and 77% in 1997).
Effect sizes were used for the comparisons of boys with girls,
Pakeha with Mäori, Pakeha with Pasifika students, and students
for whom the predominant language at home was English with those
for whom it was not. Effect size is the difference in mean (average)
performance of the two groups, divided by the pooled standard deviation
of the scores on the particular task. For this summary, these effect
sizes were averaged across all tasks.
Year 4 boys averaged slightly higher than girls, with a mean effect
size of 0.14 (a little higher than the mean effect sizes of 0.08
in 2005 and 0.10 in 2001). Year 8 boys averaged very slightly higher
than girls, with a mean effect size of 0.03 (in both 2005 and 2001,
girls were ahead of boys by an identical margin).
Pakeha students averaged moderately to substantially higher than
Mäori students, with mean effect sizes of 0.42 for year 4
students (similar to 0.37 in 2005 and 0.46 in 2001) and 0.38 for
year 8 students (similar to 0.35 in 2005 and 0.42 in 2001).
Year 4 Pakeha students averaged substantially higher than Pasifika
students, with a mean effect size of 0.50 (compared with 0.35 in
2005 and 0.59 in 2001). Year 8 Pakeha students also averaged substantially
higher than Pasifika students, with a mean effect size of 0.53
(essentially unchanged from 0.51 in 2005 and 0.53 in 2001). Responses
to the Mathematics Survey showed a clear tendency for Pasifika
students to be more enthusiastic about studying mathematics than
their Pakeha counterparts.
Compared to students for whom the predominant language at home
was English, students from homes where other
languages predominated averaged moderately lower, with mean effect
sizes of 0.20 for year 4 students and 0.24 for year 8 students
(compared to 0.10 for both year levels in 2005). Comparative figures
are not available for the assessments in 2001. Year 4 and year
8 students whose predominant language at home was not English tended
to be more positive about studying math-ematics than students whose
predominant language at home was English.
|
Overall
Trends |
|
Considering
the results on all of the trend tasks in this report, it is appropriate
to conclude that there has been no change overall between 2005
and 2009 in the mathematics performance of year 4 or year 8 students.
Between 2001 and 2005, averaged across about 200 trend task components
included in the report on the 2005 assessments, the percentage
of year 4 students succeeding with each component decreased by
an average of just over 2%, while the performance of year 8 students
was unchanged. The decrease for year 4 in 2005 came entirely from
a decline in performance on basic number fact tasks: the result
on other tasks showed a small increase. Between 1997 and 2001,
in the report in the 2001 assessments, there had been an average
increase of 4% on year 4 trend task components, and of 1% on year
8 trend task components. Putting these three trend periods together
suggests that over the 12 years from 1997 to 2009 there has been
a small net improvement in mathematics performance at year 4 level
(held back from a larger improvement by the decline between 2001
and 2005 in basic fact knowledge), and essentially no net change
in mathematics performance at year 8 level.
|
|