Analysis of Children's Written and Oral Language.

APPENDICES
Appendix 3 ; DEFINITIONS OF ORAL LANGUAGE CODING CATEGORIES
Breathing: The manner in which students took in air and breathed out was noted, with particular attention paid to audibility or obvious obstruction of airways. Students were identified as having “clear”, “moderate' or “congested/mouth” breathing.
Content: The number of reasons a student gave for their selection was recorded, from “three or more reasons” to “two reasons” to “one reason” to “little/no reason”.
Dysfluencies: Students were identified in relation to the number of halts or interruptions which broke the continuous flow of their speech. (These interruptions included awkward breath breaks, false starts and hesitations. They did not include reiterations or repetitions for emphasis.) Speech delivery was identified as having “no interruptions”, “1-2 interruptions”, “3-4 interruptions” or “5 or more interruptions”.
Fluency: Students were identified as “consistently fluent”, “usually fluent” or “halting” in their speech.
Justification: Students were identified according to their provision of substantiating comments to support or expand their position. They were identified as providing ideas that were “multiple and developed”, “relevant though not fully developed”, “vague though on the right track” or “of little or no relevance”.
Non-Verbal Cues: Students' use of eye contact, facial expression or gesture to gain attention, communicate ideas, and obtain feedback was observed. Non-verbal cues were identified as occurring “constantly”, “usually”, “sometimes” or “not evident”.
Posture: Students were observed in terms of their overall bearing and the relative position of body parts (shoulders squared, head up, hands away from face) while sitting. Posture was identified as being either “correct”, “adequate” or “poor”.
Pronunciation: Students' pronunciation was coded in relation to standard or accepted usage. (Elision or words, assimilation of sounds or syllables, or mispronunciation were all considered examples of mispronunciation in this context.) Students were identified as using “correct”, “usually correct”, “sometimes correct” or “poor” pronunciation.
Purpose: Students were identified as being 'consistently”, “usually”, “sometimes” or “barely” committed to the purpose of the discussion.
Sentence Construction: Students were identified according to their use of either complex sentences (main clause joined to one or more subordinate clauses) or compound sentences (two or more clauses joined by a coordinating conjunction) or elliptical (minor) sentences (i.e. sentences/utterances in which part of the structure has been omitted.)
Speech Clarity: Students were identified as using enunciation that was “consistently clear”, “usually clear”, “sometimes clear” or “unclear”.
Speech Fillers: Instances of speech fillers in students' speech were counted and students were identified as showing “no evidence”, “one or two instances”, “three or four”, or five or more instances” of speech fillers used to fill spaces between words or ideas in speech over the duration of their task.
Syntax: Students were identified as being “highly consistent”, “usually consistent” or having “little/no consistency in their application of rules of syntax in their speech.
Vividness: A student's use of verbs, nouns, adverbs and/or figurative language to colour or enliven their oral language was identified as being “high”, “moderate” or “basic”. (Basic language was identified as that which did not go beyond everyday usage.)

Appendix 2 / Appendix 4

top of page    |    return to Probe Studies - INDEX   |    return to Other Studies menu
For further information and contact details for the Author    |    Contact USEE