aTask
and acceptable answers |
There
were three parts to this task. The first of these asked students to
plan how to compare four boxes of different masses, the second part
asked them to describe their actions as they compared them, and the
third asked them how they would explain how to do this task to a peer.
Each of these required students to put emphasis on different parts
of their explanations and this is reflected in the majority of students'
responses. The instructions for the teacher administrator were the
same for Year 4 and Year 8 students. Instructions for what the teacher
administrator was to do are presented in bold. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Weigh
Up |
Throughout
this activity encourage the student to explain what
they are doing and thinking about. |
Place the four boxes in a row in front of the student,
in order A, B, C, D. |
1. Here are four boxes of Pebbles. They look the same,
but they each have a different weight or mass. |
Think
about how you could put them in order from the lightest
to the heaviest - then tell me how you would do it using
the balance. Don't use the balance yet. |
If the student simply says "Weigh them"...
How would they go about weighing them? |
Put
the placement mat in front of the student. |
2.
I want you to use this balance to help you work out
the order of the objects, from the lightest to the heaviest.
Tell me how you are working it out as you are doing
it and put the boxes in order on the placement mat.
|
Once
the student has arranged the boxes in order from lightest
to heaviest, record their decisions on the recording
sheet. |
3.
If you had to explain to someone else in your class
how to work out the order from lightest to heaviest,
what would you tell them to do? |
|
|
Figure
4.1: Instructions for the Weigh Up Task. |
|
The
first portion, labelled Plan, was scored as 'clear, logical, complete',
'nearly complete', 'on the right track but substantially incomplete',
or 'other'. The second portion, labelled Description, was scored
as 'all correct', 'one inversion' or 'worse'. The third portion,
labelled Explanation, was scored as 'clear, logical and complete',
'nearly complete', 'on the right track but substantially incomplete',
or 'other'. |
|
|
|
|
|
aWeigh
Up Plan |
This
section looks specifically at the responses to the first question,
which we have labelled as the Plan. As was the case for each task,
Table 4.1 begins this section by showing which students had clear
language and accurate answers. This provides a 35 general orientation
as to how students responded to the task in regard to their ability
to do the mathematics and their ability to use clear language to explain
their Plan. As can be seen there were many students who struggled
with what this part of the task involved. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.1. Clarity of speech versus accuracy of answer*. |
 |
|
Clear
Language |
Moderately
clear but vague on specific details |
Unclear,
multiple reruns, vague |
Elliptical |
 |
Accurate |
1 Yr 8
HG |
4 Yr 8
HB |
1 Yr 8
HB |
1 Yr 8
MB |
|
1 Yr 4
LG |
1 Yr 8
MG |
2 Yr 8
MG |
2 Yr 8
MG |
|
1 Yr 4
HB |
3 Yr 8
LG |
1 Yr 8
LG |
1 Yr 4
MB |
|
|
5 Yr 8
LB |
1 Yr 4
LG |
1 Yr 4
LB |
|
|
2 Yr 4
HB |
|
|
|
|
3 Yr 4
HG |
|
|
|
|
1 Yr 4
MG |
|
|
|
|
2 Yr 4
LG |
|
|
|
|
1 Yr 4
LB |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Approaching
accuracy |
1 Yr 8
MB |
3 Yr 8
MB |
1 Yr 8
MB |
1 Yr 8
HG |
|
1 Yr 8
HG |
2 Yr 8
HG |
1 Yr 4
HG |
1 Yr 4
MG |
|
1 Yr 4
HG |
1 Yr 8
LG |
1 Yr 4
MB |
1 Yr 8
LG |
|
1 Yr 8
MG |
2 Yr 4
HB |
1 Yr 4
HB |
|
|
|
3 Yr 4
MB |
1 Yr 4
MG |
|
|
|
1 Yr 4
MG |
1 Yr 4
LB |
|
|
|
2 Yr 4
LG |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Unclear
about the task |
|
1 Yr 4
MG |
1 Yr 4
MG |
1 Yr 8
LB |
|
|
|
1 Yr 4
LB |
2 Yr 4
LB |
|
|
|
|
1 Yr 8
HB |
|
|
|
|
1 Yr 8
HG |
|
|
|
|
1 Yr 4
HG |
 |
*student
identification as in Table 3.1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
There
were equal numbers of students who were judged as being accurate
as there were students who were only deemed as approaching accuracy.
A further nine students seemed unsure of what was involved, either
by providing a minimal response or by attempting to answer a completely
different question. Quite often it seemed that they were trying
to ensure that two boxes equalled the weight of the other two boxes.
The following comes from a Year 4 girl attending a middle decile
school:
Uh, Ok … what
I'd do is I'd measure two of the Pebbles in one and if they don't
equal up to the same I'd put one of the Pebbles down and get another
box and put it on the weight, and if that weighed exactly right
then I knew that that was the right balance.
The distribution
of students does not show any major trends. Five of the six Year
8 boys attending a high decile school were judged as having a correct
answer but none were considered to have a clear explanation. There
was only one Year 8 girl from a high decile school who had an accurate
response. There were equal numbers of Year 4 boys and girls from
high decile schools who gave accurate responses. The only group
of students not to be unclear about the task were boys attending
a middle decile school.
By looking more
carefully at the text structures which made up the students' responses,
it is possible to investigate whether there was any correlation
between judgements about the clarity of the explanation and their
accuracy. The following section, therefore, describes the text elements
and the common combinations found in these responses as well as
the logical connectives joining these combinations. |
|
|
|
|
|
aText
Structures |
In
the Plan, like in other aspects of this task, there were many elements
used in a variety of combinations. This may have been related to
the extended answers required by the task. It would seem sensible
for longer responses to be more likely to use a variety of elements.
Yet in the Bank Account task where an extended response was often
provided, there was not as great a variety of elements. It seems
more likely that the variety of text elements was related to the
fact that there were several ways that the Plan could be answered.
Students were told that they could handle the boxes but that they
were not allowed to use the balance, just describe what they would
do with the balance. Some students, therefore, talked about their
estimation of the order from their handling of the box, whilst others
concentrated on what they would do once they were allowed to use
the balance. As a result, the answers were quite varied. The following
excerpts provide examples of these different types of answers.
In the first
example, a Year 8 girl from a middle decile school described what
she did as she weighed the boxes in her hands.
Well, right
now, by just shaking them, umm, I think there's the lightest amount
of Pebbles in this box because I can hear them the most whilst
they're moving quite a lot and then see, I don't think that there's
that many in that box
As a response
to comments and questions by the teacher, she began with a Premise
which was followed by a Supposition, 'I think', with an elaboration,
'there's the lightest amount of Pebbles in this box'. This was followed
by a Consequence 'because I can hear them the most' which actually
preceded its Premise 'whilst they're moving quite a lot'. Another
Consequence 'and then see' follows before the Supposition' I don't
think' with its Elaborator 'that there's that many in that box'.
Suppositions are elements that are described in greater detail below.
The second example
illustrates what a Year 8 boy from a high decile school would have
done with the balance:
Umm, you'd
get those two and find out the heavier one and put that like that
and then test those two and the heavier one would go there and
then you could test those two and the heavier one would go there
and you'd keep doing it until they're all right.
This response
began with a Premise which was followed by a Consequence and this
combination was then repeated. The response was completed with an
implicit Conclusion followed by an Elaborator.
The elements
used were Premise, Consequence and Conclusions, both explicit and
implicit, as there had been in the Better Buy task. But there were
also Introductions, Elaborators, Suppositions and Physical Consequences.
The students who used these elements is given in Table 4.3 and the
elements themselves are described in the next paragraphs. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.2. Use of text elements by different groups. |
Text
Elements |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
introduction |
2 |
1 |
21 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
3 |
premise |
35 |
31 |
31 |
35 |
22 |
21 |
23 |
66 |
elaborator |
25 |
21 |
18 |
28 |
14 |
16 |
16 |
46 |
consequence |
20 |
26 |
24 |
22 |
13 |
16 |
17 |
46 |
physical
consequence |
11 |
10 |
11 |
10 |
10 |
4 |
7 |
21 |
conclusion |
0 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
2 |
2 |
implicit
conclusion |
4 |
7 |
4 |
7 |
3 |
5 |
3 |
11 |
supposition |
10 |
5 |
3 |
12 |
6 |
6 |
3 |
15 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
In
Table 4.2, it can be seen that Premises were used by most students.
However, unlike Better Buy, Motorway and Bank Account where every
student used a Premise, there were six students who did not use
a Premise in responding to this part of the Weigh Up task. One Year
4 boy from a low decile school did not answer at all but the remaining
five students gave a response which did not include a Premise. Often
these students used a Physical Consequence instead. For example,
the following response is from a Year 8 boy from a high decile school:
This one feels
the lightest and then
Q
That's the lightest, that's the second lightest and then that
one and that one
The boy began
with a Physical Consequence and after the teacher's comment gave
an implicit Conclusion describing the order of the boxes from lightest
to heaviest.
In this section
of the task, there were more Year 8 students who used Elaborators,
implicit Conclusions and Suppositions than Year 4 students. More
girls used Suppositions than boys. However, more boys appeared to
use Consequences than girls. There did not seem to be any other
major differences in the distribution of elements between the groups.
In the Motorway
task, an Introduction only provided a personal response. In this
task, the Introduction was an element that set up whether the student
was giving a personal or generalised response. As is seen in Table
4.2, there were three students who used Introductions. Two students
began their responses with 'what I'd do is' and 'what you could
do is'. These both included an Elaborator embedded within the main
clause. In both clauses 'is' is the main verb and 'what I'd do'
and 'what you could do' are the Elaborators. The use of 'you' suggests
a more generalised response than when the students uses the first
person, 'I'.
The only other
student who used an Introduction did not do so at the beginning
of the response. This year 8 boy from a middle decile school began
with an Implicit Conclusion, 'I've done it', and it was only after
a prompt from the teacher administrator that the student re-started
with 'I went like', which made it clear that this was a description
of what this student had done rather than a generalised statement.
This last example was much closer to the Introductions used in the
Motorway task which were all related to the calculation being described.
Suppositions
were clauses which put forth a proposition. There were several ways
that students did this. Using 'say', 'perhaps', 'probably' or 'you
think' seemed to set up a hypothetical situation whilst 'I think'
was more likely to show uncertainty about the validity of a result
or the suggestion being made. In this part of the task, there were
15 students who used Suppositions.
Four students
used 'say' which was coded as a Supposition. A Year 8 boy from a
low decile school used 'say' to make a suggestion which could then
be further developed in the following utterance, 'Um, just pick
any one and put them on, and then, say that that one was heavier
than this one'. A Year 8 boy from a middle decile school also used
'say' in the following extract from his response: 'say A's the next
heaviest, it feels like..'. A Year 8 girl from a middle decile school
also said, '...and then, C, say if this was the lightest then I'll
go C and D'. Another Year 8 girl from a high decile school also
used 'say' in the following, 'measure ah, say A and B together and
see which one's heavier'. 'Say' therefore was a marker to show that
it was a hypothetical rather than an actual situation being described.
In these cases, 'say' appeared to be used as an intermediate step
between only talking about the specific situation in which the student
is involved and being able to produce a generalisation which would
cover any possibility. This similar to the 'general' category that
Bills and Grey (2001) found in their research.
In a similar
manner, a Year 8 boy from a middle decile school said, 'if I say
that that's lightest...'. Although coded as a Premise because of
it being the basis for his argument, it shared many similarities
with Suppositions. There were two other suppositions which used
the expression 'you think' which also seemed to fit this category.
A Year 8 boy from a low decile school said, 'by grabbing ... um,
ah just one that you think it is' and a Year 4 girl from a high
decile school said, 'and you'd put down the one that you thought
was the heaviest'. These students were responding to the part of
the question about what would you do if you were able to use the
balance.
A Year 8 boy
from a high decile school who had used 'say' in his response, also
used the expression 'perhaps B's still heavier' which was coded
as a Premise but shared many of the characteristics of a Supposition
by suggesting a possibility. This student was the only one to use
'perhaps' in this way. As well, there were three students who used
'probably' to suggest uncertainty about the Premise, Physical Consequence,
Consequence or Conclusion that they were making. A Year 4 boy from
a high decile school said at the end of his response, 'like it won't
quite be down there, probably be about there'. A Year 8 girl, also
from a high decile school, said in a fairly disjointed response,
'okay, you'd probably put those two on'. The third use of probably
was by a Year 8 girl at a low decile school:
Ok, you could
go like that, and that one there's lighter than that one .. I
think .. yeah, that one there's probably lighter than that one,
and, that's probably lightest up there .. and
that one there, and that one there.
Q
Probably do it the same way, just put like A
there..
It is difficult
to know how much this student was using 'probably' to show a suggestion
and how much she was using it as a hedge to lessen the likelihood
of losing face in front of the teacher administrator.
A similar difficulty
in making judgements about the purpose of these elements also arose
with students' use of 'I think'. The other nine students who used
Suppositions used 'I think' to express uncertainty about the correctness
of their order of the boxes after they had weighed them by hand.
As was the case with 'I think' in the Motorway and Bank Account
tasks, these expressions can be used as hedges to reduce the possibility
of the student losing face. In these situations, the students could
have been certain about the result, but did not want to appear too
assertive. For example, they may have been able to guess that the
next part of the task required them to actually use the balance
to find a definitive answer so they did not want to pre-empt doing
this. As was seen in the example of the Year 8 girl above, it is
difficult to determine how much the 'I think' reflects genuine uncertainty
and how much it is used to acknowledge the power relationship between
the student and the teacher. This kind of difficulty is not uncommon
in research on hedges (Meyerhoff, 1987). As a result, 'I think'
was labelled as a Supposition rather than a hedge. The following
table shows the distribution of Suppositions and other proposing
devices. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.3. Use of supposition and other proposing devices. |
Suppositions |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
say |
3 |
1 |
0 |
4 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
4 |
perhaps
/ probably |
2 |
2 |
1 |
3 |
1 |
0 |
3 |
4 |
you think
/ thought |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
2 |
I think |
6 |
3 |
2 |
7 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
9 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
There
were far more uses of 'I think' than any other types of Suppositions.
Although the numbers are small, it would seem that students from
high decile schools were less likely to use 'I think'. It would
also seem that girls were more likely to use Suppositions than boys
and they were used predominantly by students in Year 8. This last
trend is interesting because it would be anticipated that more Year
4 students than Year 8 students would have difficulty in making
an accurate judgement by hand of the order of the boxes, but they
are not the ones to express uncertainty about their estimation.
It may be, of course, that there were fewer students in Year 4 who
responded to this question by estimating the order of the boxes.
However, Table 4.2 shows that equal numbers of students in Year
4 and Year 8 used a Physical Consequence to describe the results
of an action that they had undertaken. This suggests that equivalent
numbers of students chose to respond to this task by weighing the
boxes by hand.
A new element
found in the responses to this task was one which described the
consequence of an action but was not related to a Premise. These
were labelled as Physical Consequences and an example of one is
the following from a Year 8 Pacific boy: 'and these two kind of
feel the same'. Although more common in the second part of this
task, there were instances in responding to the first question where
students began with or only used Physical Consequences in their
responses.
There were also
quite varied combinations of text elements in the structures. For
example, a Year 4 girl attending a high decile school said the following:
Pick it up
and if they're heavy, leave it by itself, and if the other one's
are light, umm, if they are like this one
Q
B and, C is the heaviest and B, A and D are light and um
Q
You'd pick them up and feel the weight, and there's the second
one …
Q
The two last ones down there put them in the same one, oh no,
the big one, the small one goes down there and the big one, the
big one and the small one goes down …
This response
contains a Premise - Consequence - Premise - Physical Consequence
- Premise - Consequence - Physical Consequence - Premise - Physical
Consequence combination. Although some students used simple text
structures, complex combinations were common and can be seen in
the Table 4.4. One Year 4 boy from a low decile school only replied
'no' to all teacher prompting. The '+' indicates that the text combination
could also be followed by other elements. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.4. Use of text structures by different groups. |
Text
Structures |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
Intro –
elaborator –
premise –
consequence –
elaborator + |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
premise |
4 |
5 |
5 |
4 |
3 |
3 |
3 |
9 |
premise
–
elaborator +, + |
9 |
5 |
4 |
10 |
5 |
5 |
4 |
14 |
premise
–
consequence +, + |
14 |
14 |
15 |
13 |
9 |
7 |
12 |
28 |
consequence
–
premise +, + |
1 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
0 |
2 |
0 |
2 |
premise
–
physical consequence +, + |
1 |
3 |
1 |
3 |
3 |
1 |
0 |
4 |
premise
–
supposition +,+ |
2 |
0 |
0 |
2 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
physical
consequence +, + |
3 |
3 |
4 |
2 |
3 |
1 |
2 |
6 |
supposition
–
implicit conclusion –
premise –
consequence –
elaborator +, + |
0 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
implicit
conclusion |
1 |
2 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
3 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The
most interesting thing about the results in Table 4.4 is the lack
of any clear patterns in their distribution. This is partly because
of the small numbers of students who used many of these combinations
of elements. There are only three combinations which were used by
more than 9 students. Of these, two had similar distributions 41
across gender, year level and decile level of school attended. Only
the text structure which began with Premise - Elaborator, with other
elements following, was clearly used by more girls than boys and
by Year 8 students more than Year 4 students. This pattern is emphasised
when another 4 students who also used a Premise - Elaborator combination
are added to these results. It is interesting to note that two Year
4 students began their responses with Consequence - Premise combination
joined with 'because'. Donaldson (1986) stated that this combination
was more common in her research that than ones where the action
was joined to its result with a 'so'. Although very few students
used 'so', as can be seen in Table 4.7, the Premise - Consequence
combination was far more prevalent in responses to all tasks.
Table 4.5 sets
out the combinations which included an Elaborator. The first number
in each box gives the number of students using that combination
with an Elaborator, whilst the second number after the '/' provides
information about the total numbers of students who used the original
element from Table 4.2. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.5. Text structures with combonations including an Elaborator. |
Text
Structures Containing: |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
premise
–
elaborator |
12/35 |
6/31 |
7/31 |
11/35 |
8/22 |
5/21 |
5/23 |
18/66 |
consequence
–
elaborator |
18/20 |
17/26 |
19/24 |
16/22 |
10/13 |
11/16 |
14/17 |
35/46 |
supposition
–
elaborator |
5/10 |
3/5 |
0/3 |
8/12 |
3/6 |
5/6 |
0/3 |
8/15 |
physical
consequence –
elaborator |
1/11 |
1/11 |
1/12 |
1/10 |
0/10 |
1/5 |
1/7 |
2/22 |
(implicit)
conclusion –
elaborator |
1/5 |
2/8 |
1/4 |
2/9 |
0/4 |
1/4 |
2/5 |
3/13 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.2 shows that equivalent numbers of boys and girls used Elaborators.
However, from Table 4.5 it can be seen that girls used more combinations
with elaborators in them (37 to 29), with the main difference being
in the use of a Premise - Elaborator combination. When comparisons
are made between the initial numbers of students who used Premises,
Consequences, etc and those who used these elements in combination
with Elaborators, there are some major differences between groups.
If girls used Consequences in their responses, they were more likely
to follow them with an Elaborator than boys were. Although the numbers
are small, there also seems to be a tendency for students attending
middle decile schools to combine Suppositions with Elaborators than
any other group. Year 8 students also seemed more likely than Year
4 students to combine a Supposition with an Elaborator. However,
the number of Year 4 students who used a Supposition was very small
(3) and it is difficult to know how reliable this conclusion is.
What could be said is that if a Year 8 student used a Supposition
then they were quite likely to follow it with an Elaborator. Students
who used Physical Consequences and, to a lesser extent, implicit
Conclusions were less likely to combine these elements with an Elaborator
than with other text elements.
The relationship
between Consequence and Elaborator elements is strongly influenced
by the distribution of the Premise - Consequence - Elaborator combination.
This combination was frequently used by students in their responses
to all parts of the Weigh Up task. For example, a common response
was something like the following which came from a Year 4 boy in
a middle decile school; 'you could pick them up and see if they
are heavy or light'. 'You could pick then up' is the Premise 'and
see' is the Consequence with 'if they are heavy or light' as the
Elaborator of the Consequence.
The way that
different combinations of text elements are joined can support the
cohesion of the student's text. The following sets of tables show
the distribution of various combinations of elements and the logical
connectives used to join them together. The first pair of tables
is about the combination of Premise - Consequence (with and without
an Elaborator as the next element) and Premise - Elaborator - Consequence.
It looks at who used them anywhere within their responses.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.6. Text structures containing Premise and Consequence combinations. |
Text
Structures Containing: |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
premise
–
consequence (without elaborator) |
5 |
6 |
7 |
4 |
6 |
0 |
5 |
11 |
premise
–
consequence –
elaborator |
14 |
15 |
16 |
13 |
8 |
10 |
11 |
29 |
premise
–
elaborator –
consequence |
3 |
2 |
0 |
5 |
3 |
2 |
0 |
5 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
More
Year 4 students than Year 8 students used a Premise - Consequence
combination but only Year 8 students used a Premise - Elaborator -
Consequence combination, although there were only 5 of these. None
of these students were from a high decile school. Although 10 students
from middle decile schools used a Premise - Consequence - Elaborator
combination, none used a Premise - Consequence combination which was
not followed by an Elaborator. This is interesting because in Table
4.5, the difference between the groups using a Premise - Elaborator
combination within their response was not great. The numbers are small
thus limiting what can be reasoned from this result. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.7. Logical connectives between Premise and Consequence and
Premise – Elaborator and Consequence. |
Logical
Connectives |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
then |
3 |
4 |
2 |
5 |
1 |
4 |
2 |
7 |
and |
14 |
17 |
19 |
12 |
10 |
10 |
11 |
31 |
and so |
0 |
2 |
0 |
2 |
0 |
0 |
2 |
2 |
so |
3 |
1 |
0 |
4 |
0 |
3 |
1 |
4 |
and then |
3 |
1 |
2 |
2 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
4 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
It
appears that there are more students using logical connectives than
students using text elements, because sometimes the same student
might have several combinations of Premise (- Elaborator) - Consequence
within a response and have different logical connectives between
elements. This can be clearly seen in the students who came from
middle decile schools. This group used the least number of these
text combinations, although not by a large amount, but were the
group who used the most logical connectives, although once again
not by a large amount.
Table 4.7 suggest
that there were few uses of 'so' and these were all by Year 8 students.
There did not seem to be any major differences between the users
of other logical connectives. In this part of the Weigh Up task,
it was most common for 'and' to join Premises, with or without Elaborators,
to Consequences. It may be that boys use 'and' in this way slightly
more than girls do and that Year 4 students do so slightly more
than Year 8 students. However, the differences in the numbers are
not so great as to be definitive. This use of 'and' would be related
to the 'put another one on and see if it's um, different weight',
Premise - Consequence - Elaborator combination which was very common,
especially amongst students from low decile schools. The following
table shows what logical connectives were used between different
text elements and Elaborators. This includes the use of 'if' in
the Premise - Consequence - Elaborator combination. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.8. Logical connectives between text elements and Elaborators |
Logical
Connectives |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
consequence
– if – elaborator |
2 |
6 |
5 |
3 |
5 |
1 |
2 |
8 |
supposition
– if – elaborator |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
(implicit)
conclusion – until – elaborator |
1 |
2 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.8 shows clearly that there were few logical connectives which joined
text elements with Elaborators. The Premise - Consequence - Elaborator
combination which often used the expression 'and see if', as in this
example from a Year 8 boy from a low decile school, 'you hold it and
see if it's heavier', would probably account for the 8 students who
used 'if' between a consequence and an elaborator. The numbers are
too small to determine any differences between groups. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.9. Logical connectives between Consequences. |
Logical
Connectives |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile Level |
Total |
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Medium |
High |
 |
and |
10 |
7 |
9 |
8 |
8 |
4 |
5 |
17 |
then |
1 |
3 |
1 |
3 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
4 |
and then |
8 |
2 |
3 |
7 |
2 |
2 |
6 |
10 |
so |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table
4.9 shows the logical connectives between two consequences. Once again
'and' is the most common logical connective with another ten students
also using 'and then'. It would appear that students are not building
up a train of reasoning, as there was only one student who used a
causal connective, 'so'. In this part of the task, girls were more
likely to join Consequences together with logical connectives than
boys were. Students from middle decile schools were the least likely
to use logical connectives. There were few differences between year
levels. |
|
|