3.6 |
CASE STUDIES |
Mathematical conceptual thinking develops over time. Cross-cultural research,
according to Ginsburg and Baron (1993) reveals a similar course of development
in diverse cultures for early number procedural and structural understandings.
All students appear to develop elementary mathematical abilities in a
similar developmental progression irrespective of their backgrounds. However,
whilst research has shown that all students exhibit the same basic pattern
of development, this is not to suggest that all students in the same age
group are identical in their thinking. For a wide range of reasons –
social, cultural, intellectual, and pedagogical – similar aged students
perform at different levels on identical tasks and can explain and justify
their reasoning with differing success. This next section case studies
students’ responses located at varying developmental levels.
Cases of Differing
Levels of Mathematical Development
Q: |
Is 4 plus 3 the same as 3 plus 4? |
S: |
No
|
|
qqqq
|
qqq |
|
qqq |
qqqq
|
|
[4+3] |
[3+4] |
|
Because
they’re the wrong way round. |
Q: |
What about 4 minus 3 and 3 minus 4? Are they the same? |
S: |
Yes |
|
qqqq
|
qqq |
|
qqq |
qqqq
|
|
[4-3] |
[3-4] |
|
Um,
Because…[long pause, no explanation offered]. |
Q: |
Does
2 times 5 give the same answer as 5 times 2? |
S: |
qq
|
qqqqq |
|
qqqqq |
qq |
|
[2x5] |
[5x2] |
|
Um, they’re the same because they’re turned around. |
Q: |
Is
there a number you can add to, or take away from this number, 7 but
the number still stays the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
[long pause] |
I: |
Do
you know? |
S: |
No. |
Q: |
What
about multiplying or dividing. Is there a number you can multiply
(or times) this number by, or divide it by, so that the number stays
the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
[long
pause] |
I: |
Any
ideas? |
S: |
No. |
This year 4 student
read the cards at face value and proceeded to represent each number with
a cube. Her attention was drawn to the order of the numbers on the first
cards, rather than to the operation and the relationships which might
ensue from that operation. The sociomathematical norms developed within
a community of informed learners would enable a focus to be made on the
operations. When we look at the conjectures and explanations the student
offers in the questions concerning subtraction and multiplication we see
they are not consistent with her earlier reasoning, despite the fact that
she has again modeled the numbers on the cards: demonstrations and explanations
are in conflict. Like a number of other Year 4 students she is not able
to offer any solution to the identity problems.
In contrast, a number of Year 4 students focused on the number operations
and the relationships between the numbers. The student in Case B, following,
is in the process of making the transition between a procedural and structural
focus. His initial cube demonstration for subtraction stands in opposition
to his explanatory statements. At the time of the study he was in the
process of learning which aspect of the problem takes precedence over
other aspects and learning what constitutes an appropriate explanation.
Q: |
Is
4 plus 3 the same as 3 plus 4? |
S: |
Yes.
Because that one’s 4 and 3 equals 7 and the 4’s in the
front and 3’s in the back [refers to card]. |
|
qqq
qqqq
[3+4] |
qq |
qq [4+3] |
|
|
qq |
q |
I: |
[interviewer
prompts] So if I turn them around what happens? |
S: |
They’re
the same. |
Q: |
What
about 4 minus 3 and 3 minus 4? Are they the same? |
S: |
Yes,
the 4’s at the back and now the 4’s in the front. |
I: |
Show
me using the cubes. |
|
S: |
qqqq [4-3] |
qqq [3-4] |
I: |
How
many did you have to start with? |
S: |
4 |
I: |
And
you took away…? |
S: |
5 |
I: |
Could
you show me the 4 again? |
S: |
[4-3] |
|
qq qq |
|
|
|
qq q |
|
|
|
[student removes
4 of the cubes] |
qq |
|
|
|
q |
|
I: |
So
this is what you’ve got left? Three?
What about over here? [3-4] |
S: |
qqq |
|
|
Q: |
Does
2 times 5 give the same answer as 5 times 2? |
S: |
[nods
in agreement] |
I: |
Show
me 2 times 5 first |
S: |
qqqqq
qqqqq |
I: |
That
makes 10, does it? |
S: |
Yes. |
I: |
What
about 5 times 2? Show me using the cubes. |
S: |
It’s the same. |
I: |
Can you show me how 5 times 2 makes 10? You can use these blocks if
you like. |
S: |
qqqqq
qqqqq
You just count them. |
I: |
And
you get 10? |
S: |
[nods] |
Q: |
Is
there a number you can add to, or take away from this number, 7
but the number still stays the same? Tell me what it is and
how this works. |
S: |
Zero?
Or 6? |
I: |
If
I add 6 to 7 would I still have 7? Is there any number that I could
add to 7 and would still just have 7? |
S: |
4? |
I: |
If
I added 4, I still have 7? |
S: |
Yes. |
I: |
If I took away would I still have 7? |
|
Yes. |
Q: |
What
about multiplying or dividing. Is there a number you can multiply
(or times) this number by, or divide it by, so that the number stays
the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
Two |
I: |
If
I multiplied 7 times 2, my answer would still be 7? |
S: |
Yes. |
Clearly the student
knows multiplication facts. Although he readily demonstrates ‘two
times five’ with the cubes he is not able to provide a representation
for the problem ‘five times two’. Many other students in the
study could not provide a model for the latter when a presentation of
the former easily came to hand. The responses to the identity question
appear to have no firm sociomathematical backing and could be regarded
as ‘wild guesses’. It is likely that the student had worked
with both ‘zero’ and ‘one’ in class but had not
been confronted with thinking about those numbers in these terms before.
There is too a possibility that the student was confused over the meaning
of the question.
“Add to or take away from” may have signaled an operation
followed by its reversal, e.g., “start with 7, add 6 then take away
6, still leaves 7”.
For a wide range of reasons – social, cultural, intellectual, and
pedagogical – similar aged students perform at different levels
on identical tasks and can explain and justify their reasoning with differing
success. We consider now a Year 8 student struggling with mathematical
conceptual understanding. Most students at Year 4 had surpassed this developmental
level.
Q: |
Is
4 plus 3 the same as 3 plus 4? |
S: |
Yes. |
|
qqqq |
qqq |
|
qqq |
qqqq |
|
[4+3] |
[3+4] |
Q: |
What
about 4 minus 3 and 3 minus 4? Are they the same? |
S: |
No
|
|
qqqq |
|
|
qqq |
That’s
one |
|
[4-3] |
|
|
qqq |
|
|
qqqq |
That’s
plus. |
|
[3-4] |
Q: |
Does
2 times 5 give the same answer as 5 times 2? |
S: |
No |
I: |
Show
me using the cubes.
Show me 2 times 5. |
S: |
qq qqqqq
|
|
Oh,
[rearranging cubes] qqqqq qqqqq |
I: |
Show
me 5 times 2. |
S: |
qqqqq qqqqq |
Q: |
Is
there a number you can add to, or take away from this number, 7 but
the number still stays the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
Yea,
um… What do you mean? |
I: |
[repeats
question] |
S: |
What
equals the same? |
I: |
Still
stays the same. |
S: |
No. |
Q: |
What
about multiplying or dividing. Is there a number you can multiply
(or times) this number by, or divide it by, so that the number stays
the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
Yea,
one. |
I: |
How
does that work? |
S: |
7 times 1 equals 7. |
Mathematical understanding
is a recursive phenomenon and thinking moves between levels of sophistication.
The student is able to identify ‘one’ as the identity under
multiplication and division and can provide an explanation by example.
Carpenter and colleagues (2001) maintain that most students at the primary
level justify by example. In relation to the other problems, however,
the student does not have the mathematical language to convey his understanding,
at whatever level that may be. We can only surmise from the demonstrations
with the cubes that the student has not moved beyond the procedural level
of understanding. In the following two case studies Year 8 students provide
articulate explanations and offer a suitable demonstrations of their thinking.
Q: |
Is
4 plus 3 the same as 3 plus 4? |
S: |
Yes, because
either way you put it, you’ll still end up with the same answer. |
I: |
Show me using
the cubes. |
S: |
Three. A
group of 3 and a group of 4, equals 7. That’s this one:
qqq
qqqq
[3+4] |
|
Even with
a group of 4 and a group of 3, you’ll still get 7.
qqqq
qqq
[4+3] |
Q: |
What
about 4 minus 3 and 3 minus 4? Are they the same? |
S: |
No, I don’t
think so. Because if you’ve got 3 you haven’t got 4 to
minus. |
Q: |
Does
2 times 5 give the same answer as 5 times 2? |
S: |
Yes, because
if you’ve got 2 groups of 5 it’ll end up as 10.
qqqqq
qqqqq
Even if you’ve got 5 groups of 2,
qq
qq
qq
qq
qq
it will still equal 10. |
Q: |
Is there
a number you can add to, or take away from this number, 7 but the
number still stays the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
Yes, there’s
zero because it’s worth nothing. And if you plus zero you’ll
have 7 still and if you take away zero, you’ll still have 7. |
Q: |
What
about multiplying or dividing. Is there a number you can multiply
(or times) this number by, or divide it by, so that the number stays
the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
Um, you can
times it by 1 and that will equal 7.
And again 1 times 7 equals 7 as well. |
Q: |
Is
4 plus 3 the same as 3 plus 4? |
S: |
Yes, cos if you do 3 plus 4 first it would be the same count,
7,
qqq qqqq
cos if we changed them around it would still be the same.
qqqq qqq |
Q: |
What
about 4 minus 3 and 3 minus 4? Are they the same? |
S: |
No,
because if there was 4 minus 3 it would be 4 take away 3 which is
one.
qq
qq ‹ q
And if it was 3 minus 4
[picks up the 3 cubes]
qq
q
It would go below zero. It can’t be the same. |
Q: |
Does
2 times 5 give the same answer as 5 times 2? |
S: |
Yes
because 2 times 5 would be 2 sets of 5
qqqqq qqqqq |
|
And
you put it together
|
qqqqq
qqqqq which
equals 10. |
|
And
if you had 5 times 2, which is 5 sets of 2,
|
|
and you add
them together
it would be the same as well. |
qqqqq
qqqqq |
Q: |
Is there a number you can add to, or take away from this number,
7 but the number still stays the same? Tell me what it is and how
this works. |
S: |
Zero.
|
I: |
How
does it work? |
S: |
Well,
zero means nothing, so if you add zero or take away zero you don’t
add or take away anything. |
Q: |
What about multiplying or dividing. Is there a number you
can multiply (or times) this number by, or divide it by, so that the
number stays the same? Tell me what it is and how this works. |
S: |
Divide
it by one or times it by one.
Because 1 times 7 is just 7, and 7 divided by 1 is just 7. |
Through their explanations
and through the ways in which they represent the problems with the cubes,
these students convey learning with understanding. They reveal that their
focus is in making sense of the mathematical situations presented, and
on the reasoning and thinking which leads to a solution, rather than on
procedural rules to solve the problems. Eventually this engagement with
the underlying structure and regularities of mathematical operations will
enable the students to make links with the structure of algebra.
|