TEACHING AND LEARNING ABOUT PLACE VALUE AT THE YEAR 4 LEVEL

Chapter 2: Methodology, research design and ethical issues

aMethodology

The purpose of this investigation was to develop a conceptual model to support teaching and learning about place value. My research was informed firstly by a literature review and then by looking at how children showed their understandings of place value. A tentative conceptual model emerged from the literature and was refined by the insights gained from observations of these children. The research, aiming for descriptions of their knowledge and ways of knowing, is situated within a qualitative research methodology. The qualitative research methodology shifts the focus away from knowledge as objective and recognises it as contextual and negotiated. Therefore I used observations and interviews as my data gathering methods. These methods have been described as the tools with which researchers attempt to capture rich, thickly layered data that provide descriptions and understandings about individuals or groups (Kvale, 1996). My role as the researcher was to try to interpret and make sense of the data by looking for the patterns and themes that emerged.
         
aResearch design

In this study I analysed interviews with two groups of children. The first group was made up of children who had not participated in the ENP. The second group was a made up of children from my school who had participated in the ENP. I wanted the two groups to be as similar as possible except for whether or not they had taken part in the ENP.

For the first group I approached the National Educational Monitoring Project (NEMP) to see if they could provide me with suitable video recordings of Year 4 children working on place value activities from their annual surveys. I hoped that from these I would be able to find a group of Year 4 children who had not yet participated in the ENP. NEMP provided me with a list of schools that had been surveyed in 2001 that matched my ENP group as closely as possible. I refer to these children as the NEMP group and my criteria for choosing them were:

Year 4 children
similar decile to my school
situated in the Christchurch area
had not participated in the ENP.
         

NEMP did not have information on this last criterion. Following a number of telephone calls, however, I was able to find a school that had not participated in the ENP professional development. After explaining my project to the principal, I received permission to use the NEMP data (see Appendix 1, p.40) collected from his school in 2001. This group was made up of twelve children who completed several of the activities in smaller groups of four. These children were identified by NEMP as A1, A2, A3, A4, B1, B2, B3, B4, C1, C2, C3 and C4. One reason for looking at video data rather than just checking the NEMP report was that I wanted to observe the children closely and see cues, such as body language, that were not included in the report. In choosing this school I assumed that the children in the videos had not recently come from another school that had participated in the ENP.

For the second group of children, I decided that five would be a manageable number of children to interview. They were in their second year of participation in the ENP at my school and were selected using a purposive sampling strategy. This meant that, rather than choosing them randomly, I chose them according to pre-determined criteria. These were:

self-confidence in an interview situation
representative of a range of mathematical knowledge (as identified by the children's teacher).
Year 4 level
had participated in the ENP
ability to articulate their thinking.
         
This last criterion contrasts with views about intuitive learning and presents a problem for assessment tools, such as the ones that I used, that rely on children being able to articulate and explain their thinking (Claxton, 1997). I called this group the ENP group. In order to keep their names confidential I refer to these children in this report as A, B, C, D and E.
         
aData gathering

Data gathering involved, firstlywatching the video recordings provided for me by NEMP. This allowed me to become familiar with the types of questions and activities that the children had been asked about place value. The NEMP (2001) survey consisted of a series of interviews conducted by a teacher / administrator with individual Year 4 children from a group of twelve. Each child completed some of the eight place value activities. As a group they covered them all. I made notes of most of the conversations and actions of the individual children where they seemed relevant to the research questions.

My next step was to set up structured, face-to-face interviews with the ENP children from my school. I recorded these on video. I wanted to make the groups similar in as many areas as possible and this included the structure of the interview. Therefore I decided to replicate the NEMP (2001) activities relating to place value when I interviewed the ENP group at my school. The following activities were used:

Girls and Boys: showing 26 and 32 with place value blocks; dividing 26 and 32 by two using place value blocks.
Population: reading 4-digit numbers and explaining what the digits represent.
Calculator ordering: putting 4010, 140, 41, 4100, 104 and 14 in order of size and reading these numbers.
Number A: multiplying by 100
Number C: writing words for 3,4 and 6 digit numbers in figures; multiplying by 10
Motorway: estimating and then multiplying by 100 and describing a strategy.
36 + 29: mental calculation
Speedo: adding and subtracting 1,10, 100, 1000 to and from a 4-digit number.
  (for more detail on these activities see Appendix 1, p40.)
         

It is possible to question the validity of some of the activities by building on observations from the literature. Kamii (1986), for example, suggests that numbers such as those listed in Calculator ordering (see above) could be arranged in order according to cues other than the place values of the digits. The child could know that larger numbers have more digits without recognising specifically that place values are ordered according to ones, tens, hundreds and thousands. Likewise the 14 and 41 could be ordered according to the face value rather than the place value of the digit in the tens place. In Girls & Boys, the second part of this activity involved division with regrouping. One strategy for this would have been to exchange one of the tens blocks for ten ones. The choices for regrouping were limited by the provision of only tens and ones blocks for the children. Cotter (2000) would suggest the availability of fives as well as tens blocks. This would probably fit in better with the ENP teaching that focuses on fives from early stages of the Number Framework.

Claxton (1997) reminds us that schools tend to focus on the cognitive modes of thinking where being able to articulate and explain are valued thinking skills. He suggests that, as a result, intuitive thinking can receive less emphasis because assessment activities tend to focus on explaining and articulating thinking. This could be a criticism of the sorts of activities that I used in this project, as they mostly required the child to explain their thinking. When we ask a child to explain, we are making an assumption that thought processes are in the conscious domain. The inability to articulate reasoning may reflect the intuitive mode of thinking described by Claxton, and needs to be taken into account when designing assessment activities.

One of the assumptions of any qualitative methodology is that during a research interview, data is actively created that would not exist apart from the interaction of the participants (Silverman, 2001). It is important to allow the voice of the children to emerge and shape the interview and indeed the research itself. Therefore I tried to remain open to unanticipated answers that the children might give to my questions and to keep the interview process as informal, encouraging and comfortable as possible.

         
aAnalysing the data

As I viewed each tape, I recorded my observations in a table so as to make a written record of key points (see Appendix 3, pp.43 - 52). Reading through this table and my notes I looked for similarities and differences, groupings, patterns and ideas of particular significance. Some came up during the interview and before I had completed writing up the notes. I initially read once through what I had recorded so as to gain an overall picture and then start looking for data that related to the research questions themselves while keeping on open mind for unexpected responses. I found that I needed to re-examine my notes and the video recordings several times during the analysis process to check on some of the interpretations that I was making.

For example, in an activity that required the children to explain the place value of each digit in a four-digit number (see Appendix 3, Population, Child E, p.51) one of the ENP group seemed confused by the question and did not identify the hundreds and tens digits correctly. She did, however, at the end of the activity say that the 2 in 2495 was in the thousands column. I interpreted this answer to mean that she might have understood more about place value than the question had initially revealed. In hindsight, it would have been interesting to ask this child to go to the earlier parts of this activity. She may simply have needed more time than I gave her to think about the question. In the NEMP group, B2 and B4 also seemed confused by the same question and were not able to furnish an answer. The children appeared confused by the teacher prompt (”What does this mean?”) which was ambiguous as it could imply face, place or total value. It is not clear whether the NEMP children had a lesser degree of place value knowledge or whether they were simply confused by the question.

Kvale (1996) likens the interview process to a snapshot, where the interviewer records data at a given time. During the interviews I chose to reword questions where I felt that they were ambiguous. My thinking was influenced by the concept of reflexivity (Denzin and Lincoln, 1994) that describes the interview as a dialogue that is created by both of the participants in the interview. I would allow my own thoughts about the questions and activities to be expressed where appropriate in the interview rather than attempting to adhere rigidly to the format followed by the NEMP teacher / administrators.

         
aEthical issues

In a project such as this, researchers are guided by the following ethical principles: no harm is done, participation is voluntary, consent is informed and the interviewee retains the right to withdraw data at any stage (Tolich and Davidson, 1999). It is essential that anything that the interviewee shares is kept confidential to the researcher (myself), the interviewees (the individual teachers) and the Christchurch College of Education supervisors and examiners.

I initially contacted the principal of my school to explain the research that I had in mind and gained his verbal and written consent. The next step was to approach the teacher of the Year 4 children. After gaining her verbal and written consent for the research we discussed which children would be suitable for the interviews. The reason for this was that I wanted children who would be confident enough when faced by a video camera to provide data and attempt the place value activities. I also wanted to have a range of mathematical ability represented in the group. Once this was completed I introduced myself to the group of children, briefly explained my research and gave them an information letter and consent form for their parents to read and sign. All of the children returned their forms with parental approval to take part in the interviews.

I was aware of the implications of conducting this research in my own workplace. It was possible that a child might divulge personally sensitive information or information that compromised another teacher or child. I conducted the interviews at school in classrooms that were temporarily vacated by classes who had gone to Manual training. I tried to keep the room informal and comfortable for the children, keeping doors open to encourage the normal interactions with other children as far as possible and to make the interview environment as close to “normal” as possible.


prev page / next page

top of page    |    return to Probe Studies - INDEX   |    return to Other Studies menu
For further information and contact details for the Author    |    Contact USEE