4.2 |
CLASSROOM
CLIMATE: FACILITATING UNDERSTANDING |
Although useful
to represent children’s cognitive development within a mathematical
domain as a series of stages (e.g., see number frameworks in the Numeracy
Project, Ministry of Education, 2003b), such a hierarchical description
offers incomplete information about the processes through which children
acquire increasingly sophisticated strategies. While crucial, developing
meaningful understanding of fractions involves more than selecting appropriate
tasks that relate to students’ prior knowledge (Empson, 1999; Lamon,
1999; 2001). To explain cognitive change, we need to look further than
developmental changes to include consideration of the complexity of the
classroom processes and their impact on learning (Cobb, Boufi, McClain,
& Whitenack, 1997) – we need to consider the classroom not just
as a ‘setting for learning’, but focus on the ‘way’
learning takes place.
Several studies have looked at how better to teach fractions, employing
instructional experiences that emphasise realistic and meaningful problem
solving situations that allow children to show and discuss their informal
knowledge (e.g., Bezerra, Magina, & Spinillo, 2002; Bulgar, Schorr
and Maher, 2002; Lamon, 2001; Mack, 1990; Streefland, 1993; Steencken
& Maher, 2002). Common to these instructional experience are approaches
to fractions based on (a) realistic and meaningful problem solving situations;
(b) student’s own fragmentary and informal knowledge; (c) interactive
learning environment supporting discussion and collaborative work; and
(d) students thinking about their own thought processes when solving problems
and communicating verbally their strategies and ideas.
The move to a more student-centred classroom requires teachers to listen
to the explanations of their students, probe them for justifications,
and encourage them to share their solutions with their peers as they work
together to refine, revise and extend their solutions. Bulgar et al. (2002),
for example, conducted a year-long teaching experiment based on activities
involving the dividing of ribbons. By using responsive questioning to
elicit explanations, the teacher was able to assist in students’
development of appropriate justifications and to redirect them when they
were engaged in faulty reasoning.
Similarly, Empson’s (1999) exploration into the relation between
classroom talk and young children’s evolving fraction understandings
suggested that children’s development of ideas about fractions is
influenced not only by how their own knowledge is structured but, more
importantly, by how the context for thinking about and discussing fractions
is structured. Her study of Year 1 students’ engagement with a range
of problems involving ‘fair shares’ was more than an investigation
into the role of manipulatives and teaching activities: it was an exploration
into the ways the teacher created the conditions conducive to learning.
In order to engage in instruction which supports mathematical sense-making,
the teacher needed to attend to the interactive processes of meaning construction
by creating an intellectual community. Using a theoretical framework based
on the situative perspective Empson’s study showed how the negotiation
of meaning within the classroom played a key role in the process of incorporating
and extending students’ informal knowledge. Through classroom talk
and negotiation and thought provoking teacher questions, the strategies
children employed extended well beyond the partitioning strategies normally
exhibited by young children. Empson suggests that with discussion of strategies
it is helpful to organise discussion around a particular aspect, such
as fraction as quantities, fraction as ratios, equivalence, or the geometric
character of fraction.
The way teachers talk with students about the fractional amounts they
have created also affects the development of children’s fraction
conceptions (Empson, 2002). The establishment of correct fraction terminology
needs to be explicitly taught: Results from this study indicated that
children have a variety of terms such as ‘fourths’, ‘quarter’
and ‘1 over 4’ for describing fractions, with many of the
Year 4 students in preferring to give the answers to “How much…?”
as a whole number of pieces. Empson, suggests that teacher questions should
focus on “What is that piece called?” in order to frame fractional
quantities as something distinct from whole-number quantities, noting
that we would not ask a child in reference to a group of five cakes, “What
is that called?” Other lines of questioning that draw attention
to the quantitative aspects of fraction include asking “How
big is that piece?” or “How much pizza does one person get? – both
questions emphasising the fractional problem solving as amounts of “stuff”.
Another good question for helping children to articulate and conceptualise
the relation between a piece and its whole is “How many of that
piece would fit into the whole?” Tzur (2002) reports the use of
this iteration approach in a classroom teaching experiment involving third
graders.
Other studies specifically involving fraction learning (e.g., Ball, 1993;
Empson, 2002; Olive, 2002; Warrington & Kamii, 1998) also stress the
importance of the teacher creating an appropriate learning climate. The
more students are encouraged to contribute the intact products of their
own thinking to class discussion the more likely they are to identify
themselves as learners of mathematics with understanding. Stipek et al.’s
(1998) study, involving 24 teachers, found that teaching practices within
an instructional unit on fractions were not significantly associated with
student gains on procedural items but were significantly associated with
gains on conceptually oriented items. In classrooms in which the teacher
emphasised effort and learning, de-emphasised performance, and encouraged
autonomy, students made substantially greater gains on the items of the
fractions assessment that required conceptual understanding than students
in classrooms in which teachers did not focus on the task and learning
in these ways. They surmised that the focus on effort and learning promoted
the use of active learning strategies such as reviewing material not understood,
asking questions, setting goals, monitoring comprehension, which in turn
enhanced conceptual learning.
Stipek et al. (1998) also noted effects related to the emotional climate
of the classroom and differing formative assessment practices. It was
found that the more teachers indicated the number of errors or correct
answers on student work, the less students claimed they experience positive
emotions while working on fractions. In contrast, providing substantive,
constructive feedback on students’ work was associated with positive
affective experiences as well as with a mastery orientation to learning
fractions. An affective climate that promoted risk-taking was also positively
associated with students’ mastery orientation, help-seeking, and
positive emotions:
Students in classrooms
in which teachers emphasised effort, learning, and understanding rather
than performance and in which autonomy was encouraged reported experiencing
relatively more positive emotions while doing fractions work and enjoying
mathematics relatively more than other students. (p. 483)
There were occasions
where students in the NEMP sample volunteered that they did not like fractions,
or expressed uncertainty with procedural explanations stating that they
did not or could not understand fractions. In order that students do not
perceive fractions as the ‘beginning of the end’ of understanding
in their mathematics experience the importance of understanding and sense
making cannot be underestimated. Quick fixes such as ‘turn it upside
down’, while expedient at the time, have serious long-term consequences
on students’ beliefs about their role as a mathematics learner.
|
4.3 |
INVENTED
STRATEGIES VERSUS ALGORITHMS |
An
algorithm is a “precisely-defined sequence of rules telling how
to produce specified output information from given input information in
a finite number of steps (Knuth, 1974, p. 323). For fractions, traditional
algorithms have proved notoriously difficult for many students, (Knight,
1982), and teachers (Ma, 1999) to comprehend and remember correctly. From
our small sample of students who offered an algorithmic explanation to
Question 5, we found several examples of students confusing rote-learnt
procedures for when attempting addition of fractions (1/2 + 1/8).
While it is agreed that algorithms are important in school mathematics
– they can help students understand better the fundamental operations
of arithmetic and important concepts such as place value, and pave the
way for learning more advanced topics – it is also agreed by educators
and researchers alike, that the premature introduction of algorithms,
especially those associated with division, has a detrimental effect on
children’s performance (Behrend, 2001; Kamii & Dominick, 1998).
Rule-based instruction, often detached from children’s informal
knowledge, fosters a lack of connectedness both between concepts and procedures
and between fractions and students’ everyday lives. When rules and
procedures are devoid of personal meaning students are likely to forget
them or not always realise when to use them (Hart, 1988; Hiebert &
Wearne, 1986; Lamon, 2001). As a result, students lack ‘operation
sense’ when manipulating fractions – producing answers that
don’t make sense (e.g., answers such as 31/12
for Question 5).
In contrast, studies suggest that students can invent their own strategies
for investigating fractional properties (Empson, 2001) and operations
with fractions (e.g., Huinker, 1998; Warrington & Kamii, 1998). Huinker
claims that allowing students to invent their own algorithms assists students
to develop an interest in solving and posing work problems with fractions,
with students becoming more proficient in translation among real-work,
concrete, pictorial, oral language, and symbolic representations. Moreover,
student methods provide a fruitful source for teachers to understand children’s
developing fractional ideas and can form the basis for generalising to
more traditional algorithms.
However, harnessing children’s invented strategies into developing
productive understandings requires careful teacher guidance. Empson (2001),
in a study on children’s reasoning about equivalence, claims that
students need to do more than invent and talk about their strategies for
solving problems if transformation of ideas into generalised, symbolic
procures is desired. She suggests that the teacher needs to “maintain
a balance between acknowledging and valuing the meaning and variety of
student-generated strategies [in equal sharing], and creating a common
focus on constructing strategies that work for as many different number
combinations and problem contexts as possible” (p. 424). Specifically,
for any invented strategy, there are at least two kinds of mathematical
questions a teacher can pose (Campbell, Rowan, & Surarez, 1998): Firstly,
does the strategy make sense mathematically? Secondly, can the strategy
be generalised, and if so to what cases? For example, some strategies
may work across all number combinations, and others such as repeated halving
may work for only particular number combinations. Determining what strategies
generalise, and why, is a critical aspect of classroom activity that supports
the development of students’ proficiency in mathematics and ultimately
supports the development of students’ identities as mathematically
capable (Carpenter, Franke, & Levi, in press).
Extended opportunities, and time, for student exploration is also a crucial
factor. Lamon (2001) warns that fraction understanding development is
particularly slow at the beginning citing evidence from a classroom study
involving delayed formal introduction of algorithms: during the first
two - two and a half years, students could not compete in fraction computation
with children who had been using the traditional algorithms. However,
perseverance with children’s informal strategies resulted in deeper
understanding long-term.
Division involving fractions is the most complicated and least understood
fraction algorithm. Understanding fractions in this context requires the
ability to partition wholes (or units of different kinds); to reconfigure
wholes from parts, which is a psychological basis for the concept of inverse;
and to subdivide a part and relate the subdivisions to the part as well
as to the original whole, which is a geometric basis for composite operations
(Kieren, 1993). As was the case in the NEMP findings, children initially
use halving processes for continuous material, based on their informal
notions of the symmetry of the material, or informal measurement processes
involving the estimation of a unit and its reproduction, followed by check
and adjustment (Hunting, 1994). Further development of children’s
understanding of division of fractions is helped by appreciating different
meanings such as measurement division, sharing (partitive division), finding
a whole given a part, and missing factors problems (Flores, 2002).
Although the sharing context is extremely useful when sharing among ‘whole’
numbers of people, problems arise for problems involving a fractional
divisor – how do we visualise sharing a piece of cake with 1/2 a
person? Using the problem: “If 3/5 of a group get 11/2
pizzas, then how much would the whole group get?” Siebert (2002)
claims that we can perceive a sharing situation parallel to the way we
conceptualised whole-number division from a sharing perspective. Just
like sharing for whole numbers, the dividend (11/2)
and the divisor (3/5) correspond to the total amount and the number of
groups, respectively. The answer to 11/2 ÷
3/5 tells us how much a single group gets. The action involved
sharing the 11/2 between the three 1/5’s so
that each 1/5 get 1/3 of 11/2, or 1/2 – therefore
the whole group would get 5 x 1/2. Similarly, one can also interpret division
of fractions as a problem of finding a whole given a part. “13/4
÷ 1/2 means that 1/2 of a number is 13/4,
the answer will be 31/2 which is exactly the same
as 13/4 x 2 (Ma, 1999). Understanding these explanations
is not an easy matter and this point will be further elaborated with regard
to teacher knowledge in Section 4.4.
Several other interesting examples of children’s invented strategies
for division, based on their own or shared explorations, highlight the
propensity of children to offer solutions that ‘make sense’
within their emerging understandings of fractions. Flores (2002) reports
a teaching episode that built on a shared idea that 1 ÷
1/n = n that clearly illustrates the productiveness of children’s
explorations: Katie had concluded that 1 ÷ 2/3
= 1, (justified by “one two-thirds piece fits one time into 1”).
Rather than developing an exposition that would clarify the student’s
error, the teacher asked her students to study the situations 2 ÷
2/3, 3 ÷ 2/3, 4 ÷ 2/3,
etc. Katie used this experience to first develop a physical theory –
in 3 ÷ 2/3, there are other ‘bits’
that could make up 2/3 pieces – and then used this to develop a
successful algorithm as evidenced in her later reasoning: 61/3
÷ 2/3 means there are 18 one-thirds in 6, so 61/3
contains 19 one-thirds and half that many two-thirds.
Further examples are presented below to illustrate children’s inventiveness
and how a student’s solution method may be influenced by the nature
of the problem in hand:
- An interesting
approach that builds on students’ experience with whole numbers
is to apply the distributive law, that is, partitioning the dividend.
For example, to calculate 13/4 ÷
2 write (1 + 3/4) ÷ 2 = (1 ÷
2) + (3/4 ÷ 2) [this is the approach that was
used by most NEMP sample students in Question 5].
- Student solution
to the problem 1/2 ÷ 2 using inverse proportionality:
“There is only half a two in one, so there is a quarter in half
of one” (Pirie, 1988, cited in Flores, 2002). However, such reasoning,
while useful for this problem is less productive for solving the pizza
problem 5/4 ÷ 2.
- Rowland (1997)
reports an unusual method involving division of a whole number by fractions
whose numerator is one less than its denominator using a part-complement
representation. For example, 100 ÷ 3/4 = 100
+ (100 ÷ 3). This process has two steps, firstly
we can see that 3/4 goes into 100 units, 100 times, leaving 100 quarter
pieces to be divided into lots of 3/4’s, that is (100 ÷
3).
- A student’s
explanation of “How many portions of 3/5 of a cake can you get
from 4 cakes? is presented by Sarage (1992) as follows: You’ve
got 3/5 of a cake equal to a portion. But you can see that each cake
is one portion plus another 2/3 of a portion. That is, each cake is
5/3 of a portion. So when you want to find out how many portions there
are in 4 cakes, you can divide by the size of each portion (4 ÷
3/5) or you can multiply by the number of portions per cake (4 x 5/3).
- Another student
in Sarage’s (1992) study explained her reasoning to the problem
above as: “She started with 4 cakes. Then she cut each cake into
5 pieces. So she has 4 x 5 = 20 pieces. Then she grouped those pieces
by threes since 3 pieces make up a portion. So she got 20 ÷
3 = 6 ‘2/3 portions’. She multiplied by the denominators
and divided by the numerator.”
As these examples
illustrate, students’ thinking can be both varied and original and
not always easy to understand unless accompanied by an explanation. Just
as in the NEMP tasks, students should be encouraged to make use of concrete
representations, or empirical evidence and patterns, and properties of
numbers and operation to explain their thinking of particular solution
strategies.
|
4.4
|
TEACHER
KNOWLEDGE |
The
importance of teacher knowledge, both content and pedagogical content,
is widely recognised in mathematics education (Ma, 1999). Understanding
of fractions, in particular, is an area that requires both multiple connections
with a broad range of related but only partially overlapping ideas such
as division, multiplicative thinking, and proportion, and a broad understanding
of the range of constructs and possible representations. Given the conceptual
complexity associated with rational numbers it is not surprising that
feedback from New Zealand Numeracy Programmes (e.g., Higgins, 2001, 2002;
Irwin & Niederer, 2002) confirms teachers’ lack of confidence
in teaching fractions.
Inadequacies of teachers’ knowledge about fractions is of concern
given that reform documents urge that students experience multiple ways
of representing problems and strategies for solving them. The selection
of representational contexts involves conjectures about teaching and learning
founded on teachers’ evolving insights about their children’s
thinking and their understanding of the mathematics – one must inform
the other in the construction and use of presentational contexts. Moreover,
in order to maximise the benefits of tasks teachers need to be aware of
the advantages and disadvantages and the contexts in which each approach
tends to be more helpful. Whereas with whole numbers dual interpretations
are sometimes possible, with fractions, depending on the problem, usually
one interpretation is more helpful than the other. For example, the following
problem suggests a measurement division interpretation: “You have
13/4 cups of flour, and for each batch of cupcakes
you need 1/2 a cup. How many batches can you make? Partitive division
works particularly well when a fraction is divided by whole numbers (as
in Question 5 & 6 in the NEMP study). For example “1/2 of a
cake is to be divided among three children, how much will each get.”
To solve this, children will divide 1/2 into three equal parts, and express
the parts as a fraction of the whole. However, there are some problems
that will be interpreted by students from differing perspectives. For
example consider the problem: “John ate 1/8 of 16 hot dogs. How
many hot dogs did John eat?” Students usually solve this problem
by partitive division – sharing out the 16 hot dogs into 8 groups
each receiving 2 each. However, occasionally a student may use a ratio
conception – that is, John ate 1 in every 8 hot dogs – in
this case, they see the denominator as the number in a group rather than
the number of groups. Thus, John eats one hotdog from the first group
and one from the second group.
As well as a sound content knowledge base, Schifter (2001) argues that
teachers need additional mathematics skill in order to be able to:
- attend to the mathematics
in students’ explanations;
- assess the mathematical
validity of students’ ideas;
- listen for the
sense in students’ mathematical thinking, even when something
is amiss; and
- identify the conceptual
issues that students are working on.
Teachers with such
knowledge are not only able to ask children to explain their strategies,
they are also able to pose questions that require students to think more
deeply about the mathematics underlying both the strategies they were
using and the problems they were solving.
|
4.5 |
FURTHER
RESEARCH |
The
analysis of Pizza problem responses, especially those that required explanations,
highlight that children’s thinking is a function of both their informal
and instructed knowledge. Using these open-tasks is seen to be a useful
way to access children’s thinking. However, in order for teachers
to effectively use this thinking to inform the instructional decision-making
processes, teachers’ knowledge of both mathematics and of how students
learn must be secure: “teachers must have an understanding of how
their actions or requirements affect what is going on in the minds of
their students” (Nuthall, 2002, p. 45). Simultaneously, more needs
to be known about how teachers’ knowledge of rational number influences
their instructional decisions in the classroom, especially in relation
to the interpretation and presentation of representations.
How to achieve these aims, both in pre-service and in-service professional
development, remains an ongoing area of research (e.g., Heaton, 2000;
Lubinski, Fox, Thomason, 1998). Within New Zealand, attention of teachers’
learning within professional development programmes associated with numeracy
remains a priority. Specifically, research on curriculum development needs
to focus on ways to guide teachers’ pedagogical and mathematical
decisions rather than make decontextuatlised decisions for them: “teachers
need opportunities to learn from new curricula that are similar to the
learning that reformers intend for students” (Heaton, 2000, p. 157).
The disjuncture between students’ solution strategies using informal
and formal knowledge suggests that we need to develop a greater understanding
as to how students can develop understanding of fractions by building
on their informal conceptions of partitioning. And while not addressed
in the questions in the Pizza problem, we need to more clearly understand
how students develop a broader understanding of fractions that integrate
the various subconstructs of quotient, measure, ratio number and multiplicative
operator.
|