This report presents the findings of a three-stage research project that
was conceived with an intention to inform the active teaching of investigative
skills in primary science. The first stage involved the observation of
200 taped episodes of Year 4 or Year 8 groups of children carrying out
NEMP science tasks. The analysis of the collated observations was informed
by a review of the literature on the development of children’s investigative
skills. The insights generated by these 2 stages were then used to devise
some strategies for the active teaching of investigative skills at the
primary school level. In the third stage of the project, these activities
were tested and commented on by 3 focus groups of primary teachers.
BACKGROUND TO THE RESEARCH
A previous NEMP probe study in the area of science education highlighted
the challenge of helping teachers to “build bridges” between
existing NEMP tasks and their actual classroom pedagogy (Gilmore, 2001).
Gilmore noted that the “Developing Scientific Skills and Attitudes”
strand of Science in the New Zealand Curriculum seems to pose
particular challenges in that many teachers do not appear to actively
teach students the key objectives of this strand. This finding is supported
by the literature review of effective pedagogy for raising student achievement
in science that was jointly carried out by NZCER and the CSTER team at
Waikato University (Hipkins, Bolstad, Baker, Jones, Barker, Bell, Coll,
Cooper, Forret, France, Haigh, Harlow, and Taylor, 2002). Hipkins et al.
found that teachers tend to focus on “fair testing”, ignoring
other types of scientific investigations. This narrow focus provides a
very limited basis for actively teaching investigative skills. This situation
is exacerbated by the outdated focus on technology, at the expense of
developing explicit nature of science (NOS) understandings, in the first
strand of Science in the New Zealand Curriculum.
It seemed to us that a detailed analysis of carefully selected NEMP tasks
in which children have been recorded on video in the process of carrying
out actual scientific investigations could provide useful data to inform
the active teaching process. The proposal was initially designed at a
time when the team working on the Ministry of Education-funded Science
Exemplars project had been charged with the additional and partially retrospective
responsibility1 of developing a
matrix that included scientific investigation skills. The matrix thus
devised2 specifies the nature of
student achievement for the “Developing Scientific Skills and Attitudes”
strand of SNZC in considerable detail. In this context, we also wondered
if the planned systematic comparisons of Year 4 and Year 8 children’s
investigative actions might support teachers’ interpretation and
any potential revision of the Exemplars Matrix by providing a somewhat
more streamlined guide for evaluating children’s learning progress
in this area.
THE
RESEARCH QUESTIONS
As they were originally conceived, the research questions were:
- How does NEMP
data for actual student achievement in developing scientific skills
correlate with the relevant achievement criteria specified on the draft
Exemplars Matrix for science?
- How do primary
teachers conceptualise opportunities and challenges for actively teaching
investigative skills in science:
• generally?
• when using NEMP tasks in particular?
- Do patterns in
achievement data reflect identified strengths and weaknesses of teachers’
pedagogical content knowledge in the science skills area, and if so,
in what ways?
OVERVIEW
OF THE TASK ANALYSIS
It was our original intention to select 3 widely different NEMP tasks,
so that we could cover all of the investigative skills categories on
the Exemplars Matrix. To identify suitable tasks, we developed a grid
to match components of all NEMP science tasks to key criteria from the
matrix. This process is described more fully in Section Two. We planned
to analyse 100 instances of each selected task. In the event, the 3
tasks chosen were more similar than we had anticipated because few of
the other tasks showed the same potential to cover a number of the categories
of the Exemplars Matrix. (This comparative analysis of potential matches
between the NEMP tasks and the Exemplars Matrix is presented in Section
Two.) A consequence of the specific selection process employed has been
that we too have focused on “fair testing”, despite our
awareness of the need to widen the types of science investigations that
are carried out in schools.
For reasons that are outlined in Section Two of the report, the planned
100 observations were made for only one of the 3 tasks (Emptying Rate).
Fifty examples of each of the other 2 selected tasks (Truck Track and
Ball Bounce) were analysed as follows:
Table 1
NEMP tasks selected for this research
Name
of task/year of administration |
No.
of Year 4 groups observed |
No.
of Year 8 groups observed |
Emptying
Rate (95 and 99) |
50 |
50 |
Truck
Track (95 and 99) |
50 |
NR |
Ball
Bounce (95 and 99) |
NR |
50 |
All
3 tasks were “trend tasks”, that is the same task was used
and data reported and compared for both the 1995 and 1999 science rounds
(Crooks and Flockton, 2000), although the actual tapes observed all
originated from the 99 round of tests. A brief summary of each task
follows.
Truck
Track
A flat ramp, supported by 1, 2, 3 or 4 corks was used to investigate
the travel distance of a small toy truck. As well as altering the slope
of the ramp by varying the number of corks supporting it, Year 4 children
could investigate the travel patterns of trucks as they ran either forwards
or backwards down the ramp. Trucks ran across a provided square of material
at the end of the ramp. A three-fold builder’s rule was provided
for measuring distance.
Ball
Bounce
Year 8 students were given a selection of balls of varying diameters,
weights, and material composition. Using a long, folding ruler positioned
on a desk-top they were asked to determine which ball was the bounciest.
Emptying Rate
Children were provided with an improvised funnel made from a clear PEP
beverage bottle with the bottom end cut off and a hole drilled in the
lid. The bottle was inverted and marked at 3, 6, and 9 cm up the sides
away from the lid. Using a stop-watch children were asked to determine
the time it took water to drain when the funnel was filled to each of
these 3 marks, and then to do the same for detergent. Groups of Year
4 children and other groups of Year 8 children completed this same task.
INSIGHTS FROM OTHER LITERATURE SOURCES
The impetus for this research was generated in part by the concern that
observational data per se are not a sufficient basis on which to describe
progressions in children’s skills. Of course that is as true of
our own observations of children working on NEMP tasks as of observations
made by the team of science advisers who worked with primary teachers
to develop the science exemplars. While our analysis of the NEMP tasks
was being carried out, we also sought insights on the development of
children’s investigative skills from the considerable literature
on this subject. The literature in cognitive psychology, in particular,
has a concern to explain how and/or why skills develop in the actual
sequences that children display. It seemed to us that this explanatory
dimension would add to our research. While we cannot claim any direct
links between this literature and our own observations, some of the
findings reported do seem to us to provide useful potential explanations
for the patterns we observed. Accordingly, findings from the literature
are threaded through the next 3 sections of the report as follows.
In Section Three, the results of the analyses of the 3 NEMP tasks are
reported. The main focus is on those stages of the investigative process
at which it is possible to make detailed observations of, and comments
about, children’s investigative skills – that is, the actual
stage of carrying out the investigation. While the tasks appear to test
children’s investigative skills at all stages of the investigative
process, critique from the literature suggests that they actually represent
an inadequate basis on which to determine children’s planning
skills, or to assess their ability to draw conclusions from data. This
critique is explored in the context of all 3 NEMP tasks.
Section Four reviews the research literature to describe findings that
illuminate the manner in which children’s investigative skills
may develop over time and/or with appropriate learning experiences.
This research is linked to the findings from the analysis of the NEMP
tapes where appropriate. In Section Five we then draw these various
literature threads together to suggest some broad patterns in the development
of children’s investigative skills. These are presented within
a framework of 5 clusters of meta-level attributes, with descriptions
of the associated investigative actions that children might display.
Each cluster is accompanied by suggestions of areas of attention for
the active teaching of science investigation skills.
Section Six draws the collected findings together to describe some simple
strategies for the active teaching of investigative skills to primary
school children. Teachers’ opinions about these activities, and
their comments about their current teaching of investigative skills,
are also outlined. Finally, Section Seven outlines some areas for further
exploration and debate.
|