Reading
Rate
Students read at the “very slow” mean reading rate of
approximately 57 words per minute. An earlier study by Lesgold,
Resrick & Hammond (1985) investigated the reading rates of “phonics”
and “non-phonics” taught groups of “normal”
beginning readers in America. It was reported that both groups achieved
a mean rate of 40 to 45 wpm by the end of Grade 1, although the
“phonics” taught group took longer to achieve this.
The “very slow” mean reading rate achieved by New Zealand
students reading at band 0 in 2000 (44.46 wpm) equate with the younger
American readers in the earlier study. These findings, however,
do not support the sole use of interventions to increase reading
rate in an attempt to bring about corresponding improvements in
reading proficiency or comprehension. The positive relationships
between reading rate and oral reading performance descriptors (expression,
clarity, clause structure and sentence structure) imply instead
that these variables are separate observable problems facing students
with reading difficulties.
Error Rates
The error percentage rate was just over 9%, of which substitutions
were the most common error type, followed by omissions and insertions.
Frankel Tal & Siegal (1986) investigated errors made by “dyslexic”
and “poor” grade 4-5 readers, compared with “younger
normal” Grade 2-3 readers, in Canadian schools. The results
of this study coincide with Frankel Tal & Siegal’s findings
that nearly 50% of errors were vowel substitutions, followed by
consonant substitutions, and deletion and insertion errors. Differences
in the type of substitution errors, however, are worthy of further
consideration. Vowel and consonant substitutions formed a substantial
proportion of errors made by students in the earlier Canadian study,
while whole word substitution errors were made only infrequently.
For the purposes of this NEMP probe study, substitutions of a sound
in a sound were classified as “sound substitutions”,
regardless of whether a vowel, consonant or blend was involved.
This sample of New Zealand primary school students reading below
expectation achieved a mean substitution rate of 6.18% of words
read (the mean error rate was 9.21%), but more words were substituted
than sounds. It is probable, however, that this difference stems
from the selection of reading material for the two studies. Lists
of pseudowords were read in the Canadian study, whereas texts in
the NEMP oral reading task words were presented within an authentic
fiction, non-fiction or non-book context which provided an opportunity
for students to use context-based cues when deciphering unfamiliar
words. This difference reflects the greater emphasis placed on the
use of context cues in New Zealand reading programmes and the use
of authentic contexts in NEMP tasks.
Strategy Rates
Students paused to employ an observable strategy for fewer than
1 in every 10 text words read. Context-based strategies, especially
“guessing” or “clarifying the meaning” of
words, were the most common strategies used, followed by decoding
(typically “sounding out” of the initial letter sound
only). Coping strategies (typically an “emotional response”
or “no/masked attempt”) and mixed strategies were used
infrequently. The British study by Masterton, Laxon & Stuart
(1992) found that “normal” beginning readers in their
second year at school used a mixture of “word recognition”
and “assembled phonology” strategies to read familiar
and unfamiliar words, in a similar manner to “older skilled”
readers. This NEMP probe study observed New Zealand students employing
a wider range of strategies when pausing to decipher unfamiliar
words. The tendency for the students in this sample to rely on contextual
cues may reflect the greater emphasis placed on their use in New
Zealand schools and/or a lack of ability in the use of phonological
decoding skills. This view is supported by the strong tendency for
students to “sound out” only the initial letter or letter
blend of words on the occasions when they did attempt to sound out
unfamiliar words. These findings may also reflect differences in
teaching approaches across countries. Greaney’s 1992 New Zealand
study found that “young normal” readers achieved better
on rhyme awareness and analogy tasks than did “older poor”
readers matched for word recognition achievement, but that both
groups scored significantly better when a reading list of words
was ordered by rhyme patterns. Greaney also found that teaching
“older poor” students to use analogy-based cues was
a more effective tutoring procedure than emphasising the use of
context-based cues. Kirk (2001) found that the severe reading problems
exhibited by the 6 severely reading-disabled students in her study
resulted from difficulties in using accurate and complete letter-sound
information and integrating this information with the use of contextual
meaning to decipher words. The findings of both New Zealand studies
support the view that students reading below expectation are deficient
in the knowledge that allows them to effectively apply decoding
strategies based on letter-sound information. Similarly, Frankel
Tal & Siegal (1996) concluded that “disabled” readers,
regardless of whether they were “dyslexic” or “poor”
readers, lag behind “normal” readers in phonological
decoding skills. Remedial reading programmes would benefit from
inclusion of decoding strategies based on letter-sound information,
especially for those students whose comprehension and fluency is
compromised by very slow reading and frequent pausing.
Comprehension
Comprehension scores achieved by students in the NEMP probe study
were satisfactory, given the majority of students (64%) answered
at last 2 of 3 questions correctly. These results, however, reflect
only literal comprehension as the answers were explicit in the text.
Connelly, Johnston & Thompson (2001) used a more extensive assessment
(Neale Analysis of Reading Ability-Revised, 1989) to measure recall
of the main idea, sequence of events, other details, and some limited
inference. It was found that “phonics” taught Scottish
beginning readers (6 year olds) read more slowly than “non-phonics”
taught New Zealand students matched for age and word recognition
skills, but that they achieved higher comprehension scores and produced
more contextually appropriate errors. Further comparative international
reading research is needed to determine whether these differences
also occur in older students.
Self-correction
Rate
Students achieved a low self-correction rate of 2.46% when self-corrections
were reported as a percentage of total text words read. This approach
was used to avoid the confounding effect of measuring incidence
as a proportion of errors. Thompson (1984) cautioned against too
ready acceptance of the common interpretation that a high incidence
of self-corrections is associated with high reading attainment.
“An alternative interpretation is that self-corrections to
some extent reflect incomplete processing that occurs with premature
responding.” (Thompson, 1984, p. 53) Both data analysis methods
resulted in low self-correction rates for this sample of students,
suggesting that this strategy is not employed to any great degree
by students reading below expectation. Whether the current teaching
focus on increasing self-correction rates leads on to improved reading,
however, deserves further consideration. Similarly to the oral reading
performance descriptors, self-correction may offer an observable
indicator of reading difficulties rather than a useful teaching
focus to improve general reading proficiency or comprehension.
Oral
Reading Fluency
Students generally spoke with “little/no” expression,
while exhibiting “some” degree of control over clarity,
clause structure, sentence structure and breathing. Young &
Bowers (1995) studied the role of text phrasing skills of “poor”
and “average” Grade 5 readers (mean age of 10.8 years)
and found that “poor” readers were less fluent and expressive
than “average” readers across all difficulty levels.
It was indicated that parsing, the ability to reflect sentence structure
through phrasing and inflexion, contributed to fluency in “average”
but not in “poor” readers. Twenty-seven percent of students
in this study exhibited “little/no” control over breathing
or saliva flow, making noticeably loud noises as they gasped for
air and/or sucked up excess fluids. The finding that more year 8
students spoke quietly may indicate that speech volume control and
the awareness of appropriate speech volume levels for one-on-one
interview settings increases with age and experience. Speech language
therapy is a relatively specialised field of study and until recently
many classroom teachers have accepted that oral language will develop
naturally for most students through practice and maturation. Although
the links between speech problems and subsequent reading, writing
and spelling difficulties are recognised, more research and professional
development for classroom teachers is needed to help students overcome
this barrier to literacy. |
Gender
Differences
While boys tended to pause more often than girls to attempt to sound
out unfamiliar words and exhibit less control over clause and sentence
structure, statistically significant differences for gender were
minimal. The findings of this study challenge common assertions
that reading difficulties are primarily a gender issue. Students
in this sample, regardless of gender, achieved similar achievement
scores and brought a satisfactory range of attitudes and work habits
to the learning task. Just as Eley (1999) warned that differences
in reading achievement should not be interpreted as boys failing,
the findings of this study warn against viewing reading difficulties
as a gender, behavioural or attitudinal issue.
Text
Type Differences
Students reading non-book texts employed significantly more strategies
and exhibited less control over clause structure, sentence structure
and breath control. It is possible that students lacked experience
in reading this text type or that the fiction and non-fiction texts
provided more recognizable contextual cues. As this material represents
authentic text types that students encounter in community settings,
students reading below expectation may benefit from initiatives
to improve their reading of non-book texts.
The findings
of this NEMP probe study provide insights into the challenges facing
primary school students reading below normal expectation. Just as
diagnostic assessment is a prerequisite to the successful implementation
of programmes appropriate to the individual needs of students in
classrooms, it is valuable to approach the issue of learning difficulties
in a similar vein. Ongoing professional development and access to
a wide range of research-based assessment and teaching resources
would enable classroom teachers and literacy experts to more effectively
meet the individual needs of their students. |