3.1
ORAL LANGUAGE PERFORMANCE |
The
viewing task used for this analysis required students to select
from a visual array two photographs that they considered particularly
suitable for a poster to promote reading. Having made their initial
selections, students were asked to defend their choices. Students
were subsequently asked to identify the picture they considered
least suitable for their poster, and to say why. Although this task
provided students with the opportunity to share responses, talk
about and clarify ideas, relate their chosen pictures to other pictures
or to their personal experience, and to critique the picture they
thought least suitable, relatively few students at either Year 4
or Year 8 fully exploited these opportunities.
Although this
task was open-ended and did not require prior knowledge, some students
were quite unforthcoming in their responses: they appeared not to
relish the opportunity to enlarge on their ideas and appeared keen
to get the task done with a minimum of cognitive or vocal effort.
As was discussed in Section 2, students' overall ability to provide
full and well-developed oral justifications in support of a point
of view was found to be weak at both year levels. The following
discussion examines some of the structures and strategies that students
employed in their speech during this task. |
|
(a)
Oral Justifications
While it might reasonably be expected that oral language performance
would become increasingly fluent, confident and sustained as students
advance towards secondary school, the statistical evidence gathered
in this study does not support this assumption. Neither Year 4 nor
Year 8 students showed themselves to be strong or fluent in justifying
a point of view. Although students at both year levels could give
at least one reason to explain their selections, less than a quarter
of students at either year level elaborated extensively on their
ideas. Still fewer were able to unpack their selection strategies
in terms of the underlying messages they inferred from the pictures,
or in terms of the cognitive moves they had made in order to make
their selections. Less than a quarter of students at either year
level articulated full and well-developed justifications for their
selections. Many students responded to questions at only a surface
level. They tended to make utterances that were merely descriptions
of what they could see before them. When asked to explain or justify
their picture selections, these students did not actively seek to
extend or elaborate their ideas at all.
These findings
are in line with NEMP Forum comment summaries of student oral performance
in viewing tasks for both 1999 and 2003: |
|
Students extracted
basic meanings from visual information but didn't demonstrate the
ability to adequately explain ideas and justify their responses.
National Education Monitoring Project. (1999). Forum Comment - Listening
and Viewing, Writing, Health. |
|
The
following are examples of typically “surface” responses from students
at both Year 4 and Year 8: |
|
Teacher Administrator:
“Can you tell me why you chose this picture?”
“Because they're not reading.” (Year 4)
“They're not actually reading.” (Year 4)
“He's like…he's like like like like magic like smiling.” (Year
8)
“Well the first one…it's just like…he's not really…that one…the only
reason I chose this picture…it looked funny and just…ah yeah.” (Year
8) |
|
By
contrast, more confident and engaging speakers offered more lengthy
and complex justifications. They had more than one thing to say and
they linked their ideas together using a range of coordinating conjunctions
(e.g. “cos”/”because”, “and”, “so”, “even though”, “then”, “at least”).
The most reflective of these students also used more cognitive verbs
(e. g. wonder, think, may/might) and they used speculative rather
than definitive statements in their responses. These students were
prepared to speculate and infer about their interpretations. The following
examples are from students who were prepared to go beyond the information
given, to “read' the pictures before them. Their strategies for doing
this appeared in some ways similar to those that a reader might use
when drawing inferences from a written text: |
|
Teacher Administrator:
“Can you tell me why you chose this picture?”
P051 A1 [Picture 1] “Because the man is teaching the kids
to read and he's got the full alphabet there and they're all reading
along with him…cos all of the kids are watching him and they've
all got books and it looks as if they're having fun looking at the
picture and reading along… .” [Picture 2] “Cos that's got
the book for a frame and these people are reading books and the
boy's looking at the girl's book as if he's thinking 'oh, I wish
I had that book!'” (Year 4)
M081 A1 “Cos
this one's got three kids or a class reading and they've all got
their heads down…seem to be enjoying it...[Picture 2] and
this one…the guy, the boy's obviously enjoying it…like he really
likes the book…he's smiling. You can tell by his eyes.” (Year
8)
[NOTE: This student also adjusts his register (from “guy” to “boy”)
which he judges will suit the semi-formal rather than conversational
context of this situation.] |
|
|
High
performing students also showed evidence of being able to qualify
their opinions on the basis of available evidence and to adjust their
comments in light of their own evolving thought. This “verbal editing'
while talking was more than a simple reiteration or restatement of
ideas - these students were apparently developing and shaping their
thoughts while they spoke: |
|
“It looks like
they're very interested in reading…in what they're reading.”
(Year 4)
“Because the children aren't reading, the teacher is…or the
man is…”
(Year 8) |
|
The
following students not only speak competently about the pictures but
also show evidence of creating a story narrative to explain the pictures.
This particular strategy was noticeable among the more fluent speakers
and suggests that they have discovered the usefulness of using narrative
as a tool in explanation: |
|
“I chose that
one there cos the girl looks like she's enjoying reading…found an
interesting story that she'd like to read and I chose that one there
because the little boy is reading to his grandmother.” (Year
8)
“Cos the glow
of the book makes it seem like it's magical. They escape to this
different world with all different characters and seem to be having
a good time.” (Year 8) |
|
|
(b)
The Use of Deictic Expressions
Some (particularly younger) students in this study made far greater
use of deictic expressions than others to support their explanations.
These students used pointing and gesturing to the pictures before
them while explaining their ideas. As was shown in Section 2, the
use of this strategy was found to be markedly higher among the Year
4 students. By contrast, the more physically undemonstrative speakers
(who were generally also older children) were more inclined to speak
about their ideas without gesturing. As reported in Section 2, the
incidence of non-verbal cues was significantly higher for the Year
4 than the Year 8 group: 33.8% of Year 4 students used non-verbal
cues a great deal and only 9.6% used none at all. By Year 8 however,
only 20% of students relied heavily on non-verbal cues. Year 8 males
used them significantly less than their female counterparts. The nature
and extent of students' use of deictic strategies when giving explanations
and extracting meaning, might be a useful area for further exploration
and study. |
|
|
(c)
Elliptical vs. Complete Sentences
Elliptical sentences are an expected feature of speech, and not surprisingly
they featured highly and consistently across both year levels in this
study (64% of Year 4 students always spoke in elliptical sentences,
compared with slightly less, 60 %, at Year 8). Perhaps of more interest
is that while most students' speech was delivered in elliptical sentences,
some students at each year level, though increasingly at Year 8, spoke
in a more formal fashion, and in a way that is closer to the formality
of “spoken prose” than to conversation. These students used more formal,
written-like structures in their speech: |
|
“I chose this
one because these people are young readers. They're not big and
old like this person.” (Year 4)
“I chose
that one there like because the book's shiny and it makes it like
it could take children on a magical journey. I chose that one there
because the girl is smiling. She's looking somewhere else rather
than looking at the book.” (Year 8) |
|
|
Data
analysis revealed that the more effective speakers (i.e. those students
who achieved a high overall score for oral performance) used complete
sentences rather than elliptical utterances at least some of the time,
in their responses. On the occasions when they used complete sentences,
their speech sounded closer to formal than informal conversation: |
|
“I don't know
which one I wouldn't choose.” (Year 4)
“I wouldn't choose it because it is…they're not really learning from
reading. They're just listening to a story then just following the
words…the words and sounds in pronunciation…but it's not helping really
greatly in their own development.” (Year 8) |
|
|
This
last example also shows evidence, through the linked clauses, of
the speaker linking and developing ideas in the very process of
articulating them. This teasing out of ideas is a strategy that
seems perhaps more reminiscent of the construction of an argument
in writing than of spontaneous conversational speech.
Not all students
who spoke in complete sentences achieved a formal and effective
delivery. However. Some students who spoke in completed sentences
appeared nevertheless very unconfident in their delivery. They spoke
in a very punctuated fashion and their delivery was judged to be
very halting. The following example is from a student whose speech
delivery was reminiscent of a hesitant reader struggling to decode
text: |
|
“So/they/know/how/to
read/when/they/grow/up...because/he's/not/reading.” (Year 4) |
|
|
Interestingly,
this student's writing piece opens with a run-on sentence that carries
many features that are more typical of oral than of written communication
(word omissions, run-on utterances, clauses linked together by a repeated
conjunction, redundancy and repetition). The student does not shape
the sentence into an overarching structure: |
|
“In the holiday
[I] went with [my] auntys and my cousin and we went to drop my aunty
off and then my aunty asked us if we wanted to go to her house and
we all said yes and then we went to my aunty's house.” (Year 4) |
|
|
The
apparent interplay between oral and literate strategies will be discussed
further in the next section. |
|
|
(d)
Use of Conjunctions
The majority of speakers in the oral task used conjunctions - “because”,
“cos”, “and” - to connect or string their ideas together: |
|
“Because
the man's readin' to the little kid and the little
kid's just like reading it and so the little kid's
not really learnin' how to read so it's just lookin' at the pitchers
[sic] …or…something like that…”(Year
4)
“Because if they see that the big kids read to the
little kids the little kids would think it's cool because
they follow in the big kids' steps.” (Year 8) |
Not
all students had mastery of the use of conjunctions however. The following
student mistakenly uses the relative pronoun “that” to mean “because”: |
|
“That if you
listen to books you might be able…listen to the story then you might
go away and remember all the words and tell it to other people.” (Year
4) |
This
student appears to be using the preposition “about” as a causal conjunction,
or is perhaps using the preposition “about” in place of the preposition
“with”. |
|
Cos it's got
nothing to do about reading (Year 4) |
The
following section of this report, which examines various aspects of
students' writing strategies, will demonstrate that conjunctions can
be used in writing much less liberally than in oral communication.
Whereas in speech, repeated use of conjunctions usefully links ideas
and thoughts, in writing their overuse is distracting. |
|
|
(e)
The Dynamics of Oral Responses
Teacher-administrator influence appeared to be highly influential
in affecting students' oral responses during this task. This impact
appeared greater for those students who did not display confidence
during this task and who sometimes appeared to infer from the prompts
they were given, that they were not at all on the right track in
their responses. Some students may also have been slightly confused
by the use of the negative in the wording of the third question
in this task. Task Administrators sometimes muddled the question
”Which picture would you not choose?” into “Now choose the picture
you would not choose” or “Which one would you choose that you wouldn't
use for your poster?”.
However fully
or briefly students had responded to the first two questions in
this task, the justification they offered for their third choice
was almost invariably the same length or briefer than their first
two had been. It was unclear why students almost always spoke less
in defence of their third selection. Perhaps they associated the
notion of “giving reasons for” with the defence of a positive choice.
Therefore they interpreted the request to explain their reasons
for not choosing a particular picture as only requiring a brief
closed statement or a dismissive explanation. Another explanation
to account for the typically terse third answer might lie in the
manner of the Teacher Administrator. At this point in the task,
many Teacher Administrators indicated to the student that they were
almost done, and most appeared satisfied with a brief response from
the student even if earlier in the task they had encouraged the
student to enlarge or elaborate on their ideas. Perhaps the Teacher
Administrators themselves associated the negative question with
a brief, closed response. If so, it is possible that this reflects
a wider phenomenon among teachers to head off the demands of “getting
through” material by various strategies of efficiency, at the expense
of allowing particular students the time to explore issues in detail:
|
|
|
|
Classroom discourse
is typically controlled by teacher questions that demand quick, terse,
factual answers and leave little time for children to respond, elaborate
or reason out loud. Perhaps this explains, in part at least, why some
children do not learn how to express their ideas, formulate their
thoughts, or say what they know. Furthermore, if the teacher asks
all the questions, then he or she dictates the course of events; what
will be thought about and when. We have to ask ourselves whether they
provide the pupils with the opportunities to plan, regulate, reason
and explain themselves. Wood (1988, p. 144) |
|
|
Whatever
the explanation, this observation reinforces the importance of teachers
allowing time for students to explain their ideas fully without feeling
hurried. Student responses might be optimised if teachers increased
the amount of time given for them to develop their thoughts. Always
settling for the simple, correct answer does not allow students the
opportunity to develop their skills of explanation and justification.
These observations suggest that both the timing of questions and prompts
and the impact of teacher response on student engagement could be
a fruitful area for further NEMP probe study research. However, the
timing of task instructions, the nature and degree of teacher/administrator
rapport and encouragement, degree of task involvement, levels of student
fatigue, were among the many factors that influenced the way students
responded to the task. |
|
|
(f)
The Use of Pause, Reiteration, Repetition
All speakers used pause, reiteration and repetition to carry meaning
in their speech utterances, as well as to give the speaker time to
formulate ideas: |
|
“Because it's
not showing them reading …it looks like they're…cos it doesn't show
us that… if they're reading or not.”(Year 4)
“Because you can help people… other people… help them learn to read.”
(Year 4)
“This one here it's showing um people ahhh books, reading and all
that so they can practice…that's all.” (Year 8) |
In
the final example the student indicates that this response should
be sufficient and is, in any case, completed. Such an insistence on
closure (almost defensive in tone) often followed a relatively unreflective
response. |
|
|
Summary
Substantial variations in oral language performance were observed
across the students in the sample. Good oral language speakers used
a range of strategies to sustain conversation. They used coordinating
conjunctions, to link ideas, offered additional or connected comments,
and posed veiled questions, in order to maintain the conversation.
Poor speakers offered fewer reasons, sustained them for less time,
and had more temporal delays in their responses. Some students appeared
to have a poor command of oral language and lacked the ability to
use oral language effectively to convey and clarify meaning. For
some students, simply being asked to engage in talk and defend a
point of view appeared challenging enough. By contrast, good speakers
tended to use speech for thought rather than simply for identifying
and describing. These speakers sometimes introduced ideas by using
cognitive verbs like “wonder”, “think”, “guess” “suppose”, believe”.
They sometimes used narrative strategies to frame their ideas.
Overall, students'
oral language performance indicated a need for speakers to organise
beforehand what they had to say, and to work harder to find ways
of expressing and developing their ideas. For example, few students
summarised the purpose of their task selections or enumerated their
reasons for selection. Having a sense of the audience, and shaping
their words and thoughts with a sense of an overall purpose in defending
a point of view were skills that appeared weak, particularly among
the Year 8 students. Enthusiasm and eagerness to engage in purposeful
discussion in this task appeared more evident among the Year 4 students.
Although it
is generally claimed that children learn “spoken” language more
naturally than they learn reading and writing, this study reinforces
the importance of oral language skill development. Whereas confident
speakers are likely to speak more, and more precisely, and thereby
develop their oral language skills and expand their fluency generally,
those students who struggle to speak and who experience difficulty
giving voice to their ideas are also likely to speak less and consequently
to have less practice and less opportunity to develop as speakers,
so that with time the gap between good and poor oral language users
must surely widen. |