For
this task, students were shown a graph of the amount of money in
a bank account on six consecutive days. The instructions for what
the teacher administrator was to say and do for this problem were
as follows. Instructions of what to do are given in bold. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Put
graph and ruler in front of the student |
This
graph shows someone's bank account |
Point
to the amount of money |
Up
this side is the amount of money the person has. |
Point
to the days |
Along
the bottom are the days of the week
Have a careful look at the graph and then tell me a story
to explain
what is happening with the money (bold in the original).
|
Point
to the beginning of the graph |
|
|
Figure
6.1 Question for the Bank Account task |
|
|
The
final request to 'tell me a story…' asks for a personal response
and for a story. The statement, 'Tell me a story to explain what
is happening with the money' was not heard by some students as a
request to explain deposits and withdrawals, but merely the amount
of money in the account each day. There was no request for the mathematics
to be foremost, or for mathematical language to be used. This disguised
request for an explanation of a mathematical representation complicates
the need to display understanding of the mathematics presented in
the graph as students are expected to cross between modalities,
from the modality of mathematics to that of providing a story that
matches the graph.
A story that
included reference to putting money in and taking money out so that
there was more or less the following day as shown by the next bar
would include the expected elements. However, conversational stories
rarely include all of these elements. They are often elliptical,
expecting the listener to understand that if you took money out
of course the graph would show a lower level the next day.
In formal mathematical expression, the causal connectives are essential.
In this chapter
we first discuss the accuracy, type of stories told, and linguistic
features by age group. Then we present a section on the explanatory
text structures used. This is followed by an analysis of the hesitancies
in the students' language. |
|
|
aYear
4 students, story type |
Some
of the Year 4 students took the request to tell a story literally
and told elaborate stories, although these were not always stories
of increases and decreases in a bank account. It appeared that when
they gave only a description of the graph, the interviewers prompted
them further for a story. Few stories reflected experience with
a bank balance that changed daily. Stories were of several types,
some stories being of more than one type. Some of these stories
could be considered to be reflective of the students' backgrounds
or their interest in stories from books or the news. Several associated
the graph with only deposits or only withdrawals. In one case, this
was a story of money being raised. This may have come from experience
with school or church fund-raising where the amount raised daily
had been displayed. These stories could be appropriate had the table
been labelled 'Amount deposited in (deducted from) a bank account',
rather than the amount the person 'has'. The appropriateness or
inappropriateness of an answer depends on understanding of one word
from the interviewer. This may not be enough of a clue to override
the students' personal experience. These stories are discussed further
in Chapter 7.
Table 6.1 gives
the type of story told by each group of students. By the criteria
expected for the test, only the first column would be considered
correct. The totals for each group may be greater than six if one
type of story was given and then they were pressed by the teacher
administrator. |
|
|
|
Table
6.1. Focus of stories told by Year 4 students for the Bank Account
task. |
|
|
|
Both
save & spend or lost |
Amount
had or description of graph only |
Add
only |
Deduct
only |
Story
without reference to changes on graph |
No
explanation |
 |
Yr
4 Pacific low decile boys |
1 |
1 |
|
2 |
2 |
|
Yr 4 Pacific
low decile girls |
|
1 |
2 |
2 |
1 |
|
Yr 4 non-Pacific
low decile boys |
2 |
3 |
|
|
|
1 |
Yr 4 non-Pacific
low decile girls |
|
3 |
1 |
2 |
|
|
Yr 4 high
decile boys |
2 |
4 |
|
|
|
|
Yr 4 high
decile girls |
2 |
4 |
|
|
|
|
 |
Total
|
7 |
16 |
3 |
6 |
3 |
1 |
|
|
|
Thus
13 of the 36 students did not give the type of story that was expected.
All of these were lower decile students. Nine of these were Pacific
and four were non-Pacific students. These differences between economic
groups were highly significant (p=0.00029)
Seven students
did as the question expected and made up a story to explain the
change of the graph. Sixteen students simply reported what had happened
on the graph with no causal statement. This response was most common
among the high decile students and least common among the Pacific,
low decile students. This suggests that students who were mostly
of the same ethnic group as the interviewers believed that the mathematics
of graph reading was what was important, not the story.
There were marked
differences in the type of stories told by these Year 4 students.
Three of 12 Year 4 Pasifika boys from low decile schools mentioned
robbers or stealing in their stories as did one Year 4, high decile
girl. Stories of lower decile children included 'they borrowed it
because they had no money, they needed it to buy food, to buy lunch
for their daughter, for school' (Pasifika girl); 'mum hates going
to the bank' (Pasifika boy), 'when my dad gets paid, yeah, he'll
put money in the bank for me'(Pasifika boy) and money owned by 'the
people' (non-Pasifika boy, Pasifika boy).
Three Year 4
boys from high decile schools talked of the money going up and down,
possibly treating it like the share market or possibly just referring
to the graph, thus ignoring the story shell expected by the final
question. They commented that the graph went up or down but did
not attribute an external agent to these changes. An example of
this was:
Well, the
money, on Monday it is, it was on ten dollars, then on Tuesday
it grew (unintel) it went to twenty dollars, and on Wednesday
it's still on twenty dollars, then Thursday it lowered down, and
it was (pause) on fifteen dollars, then on Friday it was fifteen
dollars again and on Saturday it was on ten dollars.
Two high decile
girls treated it like an exercise in story telling. One started
her story 'Once there was a little boy named James and he had lots
of money…'. The other included specific purchases made, as in 'he
went to the store and bought some sugar and some cherries'. These
story telling skills are similar to those encouraged in school,
where emphasis is placed on writing and editing good stories as
an aspect of literacy. For these girls, the story telling aspect
seems to have been more important than the mathematics in the representation.
|
|
|
aYear
4 Linguistic features |
Agents
|
The
agents in the stories told were other people, not the student. One
girl from a high decile school asked 'how do I know if it is a man
or a girl?' This was one indication that most students did not see
bank accounts as something that they owned or looked at regularly.
When 'I' was used it was usually in conversation, as in 'I don't
know', or 'I mean'. Within the explanatory story, only one child
used 'I' as the agent of her story, and this was a modification
of a clause that started 'Monday we had, I had ten dollars'. Agents
were usually the same throughout a story, although a story might
use 'the people' initially and then 'they' for later references
to the person. Counting only the main agent in a story, 'they' was
most common (8) followed by 'he' (4). Other agents used were: the
person: 2; the people: 2; someone: 3; robber 2; my dad, the man,
and she.
For the students
who reported on the characteristics of the graph, bars or amounts
of money were referred to as 'it'. This occurred in 11 cases, sometimes
in conjunction with the personal agents listed above. |
|
|
Actions
|
Many
of the verbs used were specific to money (19%). These included 'put
in', 'stole', 'saved', 'spent', 'borrowed', 'costed' [sic], 'earns'
and 'gets paid'. All of these would be specific to the context and
inappropriate for an identical graph that was, for example, of rainfall.
They indicate that students were attending to the story context
that was given to them. Some of these verbs, like 'spent' were consistent
with the mathematical comparisons required while others were not.
More common
were verbs that referred directly to the comparison of the height
of columns (26%). Some of these were not strictly comparative. These
included 'grew', 'went up'. Others might be considered transitional
to mathematical expressions, like 'goes bigger' and 'goes smaller'.
Others, all used by one student, refer more conventionally to mathematical
comparison. These were 'is shortest', 'is in the middle', 'is highest',
'is the same' (Pasifika boy from low decile school).
Other verbs
were non-mathematical, with the largest proportion being static
(36%). There was only one example of a mathematical verb. This was
'makes', used to mean equals (Pasifika boy from low decile school).
|
|
|
Comparisons
|
Despite
the limited use of direct verbs of comparison, 21 students did express
this concept. This differed by group, with 3 of 12 Pasifika, low decile
students expressing comparison, 8 of 12 non-Pacific, low decile students
doing so, and 10 of 12 high decile students expressing comparison.
There was almost no difference between girls and boys using some form
of comparison, so the sexes were combined. The three groups were then
compared using a «2 test. The probability of this distribution, X2=
8.9144, p = 0.0115955. The difference between high decile and both
low decile groups was significant at the 0.05 level (p = 0.034) but
the difference between low decile non-Pacific and low decile Pacific
students was not significant, using Fisher's exact test (p = 0.0559).
|
|
|
Abstractions
versus specific numerical amounts |
In these students' answers, there were only a few instances of generalisations
like 'some', 'any', or 'much' (8). There were a great many instances
of specific numbers (138), which were used as students read the graphs.
|
|
|
Connectives
|
As
stated above, in a story about this graph one would have expected
to hear some causal connectives such as 'the next day there was
the same amount of money because he didn't add any or take any out'
In fact few
Year 4 students used causal connectives. Only 14% of the connectives
were causal, primarily either 'because' or 'so'. These were used
by nine students, spread across five subgroups. They were used by
both boys and girls of all three main subgroups. These students
rarely used more than one or two causal connectives.
The largest
proportion of connectives (84%) was additive ones, connecting the
stories for each column in the graph or part of the story. These
connectives were 'and' (54%), 'then' (12%), and 'and then' (20%).
Other connectives used, in small numbers, were 'but' and 'when'
(1% each). The discrepancy between causal and additive connectives
was found in every group of students. There was little correlation
between the length of story told and the variety of connectives
used. For example, the high decile girls, all told extended stories
that had episodes connected by 'and'. Only one of these students
used the causal 'so'. |
|
|
Year
8 students, story type |
The
explanations told by Year 8 students were more focused, showing a
clearer understanding of both the mathematical explanation required
and of what happened in bank accounts. Table 6.2 gives the number
of students from each group giving various types of answers. As for
Table 6.2, only the first column would be correct by the standards
set by NEMP, but the second column also were accurate mathematically.
|
|
|
|
Table
6.2. Focus of stories told by Year 8 students for the Bank Account
task. |
|
|
|
Both
save & spend or lost |
Amount
had or description of graph only |
Add
only |
Deduct
only |
Story
without reference to changes on graph |
No
explanation |
 |
Yr
8 Pacific low decile boys |
2 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
|
|
Yr
8 Pacific low decile girls |
2 |
4 |
|
|
|
|
Yr
8 non-Pacific low decile boys |
3 |
1 |
1 |
|
|
1 |
Yr
8 non-Pacific low decile girls |
1 |
4 |
1 |
|
|
|
Yr
8 high decile boys |
4 |
2 |
|
|
|
|
Yr
8 high decile girls |
3 |
3 |
|
|
|
|
 |
Totals |
15 |
16 |
3 |
1 |
|
1 |
|
|
|
Only
five of the 36 students did not describe both the increases and
decreases on the graph or give an explanation involving both putting
money in and taking it out. As was the case for the Year 4 students,
all of these were low decile students. This difference between high
and low decile students was significant (p = 0.01031).
Year 8 students
were significantly better at giving an appropriate answer - either
a story closely tied to the graph or a mathematical description
of the graph ¬- than were the Year 4 students, at the 0.05 level
(p = 0.01717).
There were instances
in these interviews, as in those for the Year 4 students, where
teacher administrators prodded for more of a story when students
only gave what happened on the graph.
There was far
less difference in the type of stories told by these older students
than by Year 4 students, indicating that these students had a clearer
idea of what was expected of them, and a better idea of what happened
in bank accounts. There was no obvious difference in the type of
stories told by students from different ethnic or economic backgrounds.
When the Year
8 students told stories, they were more closely allied to what might
have happened in a bank account than were those of the Year 4 students.
For example,
He's ah just
got his bank account and he got ten dollars with it, his um mum
put in another ten dollars and he's got twenty dollars, but he
ah, didn't do all the jobs in his house so um, and he wanted to
buy something, he didn't get his pocket money for not doing the
jobs in the house so he took some out of his money, and ah he
left the his bank account alone for another day, and then he decided
he'd that he um, wanted something else which was better than the
last one and so he got out some more money.(non-Pasifika boy)
There was one
instance of a non-Pasifika girl from a low decile school starting
her story with a story-telling convention. This was 'Well, one day
Fred…' There was also one instance of a student asking the interviewer,
'Do I have to tell who they are?' (low decile, non-Pasifika girl).
This is similar to the Year 4 student who asked 'how do I know if
it is a man or a girl?'(girl from high decile school). There were
no instances of robbers or of personal stories that reflected the
students' families' experience with banks. The instances of stories
telling only how much was added or how much was taken out might
be accurate, if the graph had been labelled 'Amount deposited in
(deducted from) a bank account daily', as would those of the Year
4 students. |
|
|
aYear
8, Linguistic elements |
Agent
|
The
agents of students' stories were other people, as they were for
Year 4 students, or the columns on the graph. Instances of the use
of 'I' were related to personal, conversational statements like
'what should I say?'. There was one instance of 'I' as the agent
within the story-telling conventions where a quotation was part
of the story.
he, um, actually
um, had ten dollars in the, bank account and, he thought oh, he
got, he got his next allowance or pocket money or something, and
then like, so, thought well I should deposit ten dollars, (Year
8 boy from high decile school).
All students
who gave explanations used a name or pronoun for the person in the
story (usually he or they); a pronoun referring to the graph (usually
'it') or an existential agent, as … 'on Monday there was ten dollars…'
(Year 8 non-PI boy). When
the main agent used by a particular student was analysed, 25 students
used 'he', 'she', 'they' or people as agent; 9 used 'it', the day
of the week, or an existential 'there' referring to the column height;
and 2 did not include an agent in their answer. There was little
difference between ethnic or economic groups or genders. |
|
|
Action
|
Of
all the verbs used, 25% specifically related to money, like 'spent',
15% showed increase or decrease; 18% were non-mathematical action;
and 40% were static or sensing verbs. Only two verbs, 'add up' and
'made' for equals, were considered mathematical. |
|
|
Comparisons
|
Eleven
students made comparisons, using terms such as 'more', 'same' and
'higher'. However, 28 students made comparisons of some sort, using
phrases like 'went up', 'put more in', 'it rose' and 'different amounts'.
Of the students who did not express comparison directly, some expressed
it indirectly through their story, saying what was saved and what
was spent. Under both the broad and the more strict definitions of
comparison students from high decile schools use this explanation
in the majority of cases, but there is little overall difference.
|
|
|
Abstractions
and specific numbers |
There
were 56 instances of abstractions such as 'it', 'any', 'some' and
'much' and 137 instances of specific amounts of money being mentioned.
Although the total number of words expressing specific amounts of
money is very similar for Year 4 and Year 8 students, the Year 8 students
used more indefinite abstractions than did the younger students. |
|
|
Connectives |
In comparison to the Year 4 students, more Year 8 students used
causal connectives. In this age group 22% of all connectives were
causal. Some students used a variety of causal and additive connectives.
The following example includes this variety, and has the connectives
in bold. ..
So like for
each day or? [Q] Okay, okay...um .. m Monday the amount of money
was ten dollars, Tuesday and Wednesday it were quite high, and
there were twenty dollars, Thursday it wasn't not that great but
better than Monday, and Saturday was the same amount as Monday.
[Q]That Tuesday's and Wednesday's had the most amount of money
in the bank account, and Saturday's and Monday's don't have that
much money. [Q] Ah, because Monday's like the start of the week
and probably from Sunday they probably spent money from their
account and then spent some more money on Monday and since it's
Saturday, Saturday's like a shopping day and um, they probably
went shopping and bought quite a lot of stuff so they didn't have
much money in their bank account, and they probably work on Tuesdays
and Wednesdays more than they do on Mondays and Fridays. (Year
8 girl from high decile school)
Of the Year
8 students, 5 students from high decile schools, 5 non-Pacific students
from low decile schools, and 1 Pacific student from a low decile
school used causal connectives in their responses. 7 students from
high decile schools used additive connectives only, as did 6 Pacific
students from low decile schools. Ten Pacific students used only
additive connectives. The significant difference here is between
non-Pacific students, both high and low decile, and Pacific students
(p = 0.00536). Logical connectives are also discussed in relationship
to combinations of text elements in the next sections. |
|
|
aAccurate
responses and clear language |
This
task was different to the other tasks in that students were asked
to provide a story about a graph. This caused some confusion for many
students. It was expected that the students would be able to look
at the graph and then describe a logical reason for the change in
the amounts held in the bank account. Instead, older students often
simply described the amounts of money held in the account on each
of the days. Many of the younger students seemed to be completely
unaware of what a bank account was but were much more willing to make
up a story. This meant that the stories often had little to do with
the actual amounts and more to do with robbers. As such, it was not
easy to divide students into those with appropriate or inappropriate
answers. However, clarity of language could be determined. This can
be seen in Tables 6.3 to 6.5. |
|
|
|
Table
6.3. Clarity of language versus type of story/description told*. |
|
|
|
Clear
language |
Moderately
clear but vague on specific details |
Unclear,
multiple reruns, vague |
Elliptical
|
total
|
Boys |
Girls |
Boys |
Girls |
Boys |
Girls |
Boys |
Girls |
 |
Save/spend
Year 8 |
1
n-PLD |
1
HD |
1
HD |
1
HG |
|
|
|
|
12 |
2
PLD |
|
2
nPLD |
1
PLD |
|
|
|
|
3
HD |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
 |
Save/spend
Year 4 |
1 nPLD |
1 HD |
|
1HD |
1HD |
|
|
|
6 |
2 HD |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
 |
Graph
changes only Year 8 |
1 HD |
4 nPLD |
2 nPLD |
1 HD |
- |
- |
1 nPLD |
1 nPLD |
19 |
|
3 PLD |
1 PLD |
1 PLD |
|
|
1HD |
|
|
3 HD |
|
|
|
|
|
|
 |
Graph
changes only Year 4 |
2 nPLD |
2 HD |
|
2 HD |
|
2 nPLD |
|
|
14 |
2 PLD |
|
|
|
|
1 PLD |
|
|
3 HB |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
 |
Only
add or only deduct Year 8 |
1
nPLD |
1
nPLD |
|
|
|
|
|
|
4 |
2
PLD |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
 |
Only
add or only deduct Year 4 |
|
3 PLD |
1 PLD |
1 nPLD |
|
|
|
|
7 |
|
|
1 HD |
1 HD |
|
|
|
|
 |
Other
Year 8 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
|
|
1 PLD |
|
1 |
 |
Other
Year 4 |
|
|
|
|
1 nPLD |
|
3 PLD |
3 nPLD |
9
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
2 PLD |
 |
*Codes
for student identification as in Table 3.1 |
|
|
|
|
Table
6.4. Summary of accuracy and clarity of language by ethnographic
categories. |
|
|
|
Gender |
Year |
Decile |
Ethnicity |
Boys |
Girls |
Boys |
Girls |
High
Decile |
Low
Decile |
Low
Pacific |
Low
non-Pacific |
 |
Clear
Language / accurate* |
17 |
14 |
13 |
18 |
16 |
15 |
7 |
8 |
Clear
Language / wrong |
3 |
4 |
4 |
3 |
1 |
7 |
5 |
2 |
 |
*either
save/spend or description of graph |
|
|
|
|
Table
6.5. Summary of unclear or elliptical language by accurate and
inaccurate responses by ethnographic categories. |
|
|
|
Gender |
Year |
Decile |
Ethnicity |
Boys |
Girls |
Boys |
Girls |
High
Decile |
Low
Decile |
Low
Pacific |
Low
non-Pacific |
 |
Unclear
or elliptical / accurate |
3 |
4 |
4 |
3 |
2 |
5 |
1 |
2 |
Unclear
or elliptical / wrong |
5 |
5 |
9 |
1 |
- |
10 |
6 |
4 |
 |
|
|
|
These
tables suggest that the students most likely to be accurate and
use clear language are Year 8 boys from high decile schools, but
that the differences between genders and deciles are not strong.
Students who used unclear language and were accurate came from all
groups, although more came from low decile than from high decile
schools. The students most likely to be wrong and have unclear or
elliptical language were Year 4 students from low decile schools
of either ethnicity.
Among the Year
8 students, girls of all groups were more likely to use clear language
when they told a story just based on the graph changes than were
boys, but boys were more likely to tell a clear story based on saving
and spending than were girls. An example of a girl's story describing
the graph alone was the following:
Umm ... on
Monday they have ten dollars, Tuesday and Wednesday they have
twenty dollars, Thursday and Friday they have fifteen dollars,
and then on Saturday they go back to ten dollars.
An example of
a story of save/spend from a boy from a high decile school was:
On Monday
whoever had ten dollars, and then on Tuesday they could have got
their pocket money or got paid or something, it went up to twenty
dollars they didn't spend any money on Wednesday, then they spent
.. oh, is it fif, sixteen .. it looks as if they spent four dollars,
so they had sixteen dollars, then they didn't spend any money
on Friday, and then they spent some money on Sun, Saturday, which
means they had eleven dollars left, or ten, hold on .. yeah ten
dollars.
Pacific boys
from low decile schools were also likely to use clear language when
either giving a save or spend story or when providing an add or
deduct story. The following is an example of these types of stories.
On Monday
they only have ten dollars, but on Tuesday it rises, and on Wednesday
it still stays the same, then on Thursday, they probably spent
a little and it went little, then on Friday stayed the same, and
on Saturday it went back to ten dollars.
As can be seen
from the last example, the type of story is not always easily categorised,
with some containing elements of more than one kind. This last example
is considered to be predominantly about the process of getting bigger
or smaller but does mention spending as well as making oblique references
to the graph, through the use of 'it'.
The types of
Premises in the text structures were related strongly to the type
of story. The text structures are discussed in more detail in the
next sections. |
|
|
aText
Structure |
The
text elements that the students used in responding to this task
were predominantly Premise and Consequence. However, like the Weigh
Up task, there were examples of students using other elements such
as Conclusions, Elaborators and Suppositions. The different elements
are described in the next paragraphs.
Three students
used Premises followed by an implicit Conclusion. Two were Pacific
girls and the other was a boy from a high decile school. The Conclusions
used were implicit: 'that's how much money they got … and that's
all'; 'And the large money is twenty dollars .. second one is fif,
six, seventeen and a half, that one is ten dollars, it's finished';
and '...Money's going um, it starts at ten and it's going up higher
…. Hmm, that's all'. This final clause was added after a comment
by the teacher administrator.
Different types
of Premises were used in the responses. However, often a range of
Premises was used making it difficult to code the responses definitively
as was done with the responses to the Motorway task. The following
example comes from a Year 4 student.
Um, there's
ten dollars on Monday, twenty dollars on Tuesday and Wednesday,
and fifteen dollars on Thursday and Friday .. and, ten dollars
on Saturday. Q Um, ah, they might have only got ten dollars and
then, then the next day they put another ten dollars onto it ..
and then on, then on, then on Thursday, um, someone might have
stole some of their money, and on, and .. and on Friday they didn't
bring any money in, so then another bank robber came in and stole
all their money again.
'There's ten
dollars on Monday' was coded as a monetary Premise, as it was based
on the money, while 'they might have only got ten dollars' was coded
as a personal Premise, as it had a person (although unidentified)
as the main actor in the clause. This next example comes from a
Year 8 student:
...Umm, started
off low and then going high and then going down again.
Q
Um, she's got a raise, and then she got dropped down again.
In the first
sentence, all the clauses were coded as 'graphical' Premises, as
they are all referring to the graph. However, after the teacher's
comment or question, the next sentence contains two personal Premises.
|
|
|
|
Table
6.6. Distribution of students using the majority of one kind
of Premise. |
|
|
Premise |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
graphical
premise |
9 |
10 |
11 |
8 |
7 |
5 |
7 |
19 |
personal
premise |
23 |
22 |
23 |
22 |
14 |
15 |
16 |
45 |
monetary
premise |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
2 |
personal
/ graphical premise |
3 |
0 |
0 |
3 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
3 |
|
|
|
Table
6.6 shows that even when the texts are categorised according to the
type of Premise mostly used, there seems to be no differences between
the groups using them according to gender, age, ethnicity or school
decile level. |
|
|
|
Table
6.7. Use of text structures by different groups.
|
|
|
Text
Structures |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
premise |
15 |
17 |
15 |
17 |
11 |
10 |
11 |
32 |
premise
– conclusion |
2 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
1 |
3 |
premise
– elaborator – premise |
4 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
3 |
1 |
1 |
5 |
premise
– elaborator + other elements |
4 |
1 |
3 |
2 |
3 |
1 |
1 |
5 |
premise
– consequence |
1 |
4 |
4 |
1 |
1 |
3 |
1 |
5 |
premise
– consequence – elaborator + other elements |
3 |
3 |
3 |
3 |
1 |
1 |
4 |
6 |
premise
– consequence – premise |
3 |
3 |
4 |
2 |
2 |
3 |
1 |
6 |
premise
– consequence – premise – elaborator |
3 |
3 |
2 |
4 |
0 |
3 |
3 |
6 |
premise
– supposition – elaborator + other elements |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
|
|
|
Table
6.7 provides information on the distribution by student groups of
the different text structures. Almost half of the students gave responses
which only included Premises. These students seemed to be evenly distributed
across groups. Table 6.8 shows the distribution of students who had
a Premise - Consequence combination within their responses. It would
seem that, apart from a tendency for students from high decile schools
to be more likely to use this combination of elements, there seems
to be no clear distinction between the groups. |
|
|
|
Table
6.8. Text structures with Premise and Consequence.
|
|
|
Text
Structures including : |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
premise
– consequence |
14 |
14 |
16 |
12 |
7 |
9 |
12 |
28 |
|
|
|
In
the responses which included a consequence, there were 6 instances
of these preceding the Premise, such as 'and Friday he had a day off
because he was sick'. As was the case in responses to the other tasks,
these were rare occurrences. The distribution of students using this
combination is given in Table 6.9. |
|
|
|
Table
6.9. Uses of Consequences preceding the Premise by different
groups . |
|
|
Text
Structures including : |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
consequence
– premise |
3 |
3 |
3 |
3 |
1 |
4 |
1 |
6 |
|
|
|
Although
the number of students who use this combination of elements was
small, it was most commonly used by students from low decile schools,
and not in Pacific communities. Interestingly, three students joined
these clauses with 'because', one student used 'when', while the
remaining two students did not use any logical connector.
As with the
Weigh Up task, clauses which set up propositions were labelled as
Suppositions. In two cases, 'say' was used by students to indicate
that the specific example which followed was not necessarily exact.
The first example of this came from a Year 8 girl at a low decile
school who was not a Pacific Islander. At the end of her response
she said, 'and then on Saturday it .. dropped .. to .. say about
..ten dollars.' In the other example 'say' was used to suggest that
the series of actions outlined were hypothetical; '....Well, if
this is the amount of money, and these are the days, say he came
into the bank on Monday, and he like put ten dollars into his bank
account then it shows that he's got ten dollars'. Suppositions were
more common in the Weigh Up task and are discussed in more detail
in that chapter.
The other element
in this task was an Elaborator. Most often this was a relative clause
which followed a verb to do with a mental action such as 'decide'
or 'means', or a relative clause providing extra information about
a previous clause or which was embedded within the main clause and
provided extra information about an actor or an event. The following
is an example from a Year 4 Pacific student attending a low decile
school: 'the man that's stealing the money was come every week'.
'That's stealing the money' provides more information about 'the
man'. Table 6.10 provides information about the use of Elaborators
with Premises and Consequences. |
|
|
|
Table
6.10. Responses containing combinations with Elaborators.
|
|
|
Text
Structures containing : |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
premise
– elaborator |
12/36 |
6/36 |
8/36 |
10/36 |
6/24 |
5/24 |
7/24 |
18/72 |
consequence
– elaborator |
6/17 |
3/17 |
5/19 |
4/15 |
3/8 |
0/13 |
6/13 |
9/34 |
|
|
|
There
were eighteen students who used Elaborators with Premises, either
embedded within or following immediately afterwards. Twelve of the
eighteen students were girls, but there appeared to be no clear
trends between any of the other groups. As well as elaborating Premises,
they could also be found less commonly elaborating Consequences.
The following
is an example from a Year 8 boy from a high decile school: 'he got
his next allowance or pocket money or something, and then like,
so, thought well I should deposit ten dollars', where 'well, I should
deposit ten dollars' is an Elaborator of 'thought'. There were nine
students who included an Elaborator with a consequence. Six of these
were girls and six were from high decile schools. The following
two tables provide information on the logical connectives which
were used in the responses to this task. Given that many of the
students doing this task gave a narrative either of someone adding
or subtracting money from their bank account or told about the various
features of the graph in a logical sequence, it could be predicted
that there would be a variety of logical connectives used. This
can be seen in the following example from a Year 8 student.
He's ah just
got his bank account and he got ten dollars with it, his um mum
put in another ten dollars and he's got twenty dollars, but he
ah, didn't do all the jobs in his house so um, and he wanted to
buy something, he didn't get his pocket money for not doing the
jobs in the house so he took some out of his money, and ah he
left the his bank account alone for another day, and then he decided
he'd that he um, wanted something else which was better than the
last one and so he got out some more money.
However, based
on Grice's pragmatic maxim of orderliness, Peterson and McCabe (1991)
suggested that 'the act of stating or describing one event before
the other is automatically presumed to mean that the event mentioned
first actually occurred first' (p. 34). The following tables, therefore,
also provide information on the students who do not use a logical
connective between a Premise and a Consequence or between two Premises.
|
|
|
|
Table
6.11. Distribution of logical connectives between a Premise
and a Consequence. |
|
|
Logical
Connectives |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
and |
5 |
7 |
6 |
6 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
12 |
and
then |
2 |
1 |
3 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
3 |
then |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
when |
1 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
2 |
since |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
because |
2 |
2 |
3 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
4 |
so |
3 |
5 |
3 |
5 |
0 |
2 |
6 |
8 |
but |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
if |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
No
LC |
7 |
4 |
3 |
8 |
3 |
2 |
6 |
11 |
|
|
|
Between
a Premise and a Consequence a range of logical connectives were used.
The most common one as has been the case in all of the tasks was 'and',
which used by a sixth of the students. However, almost a similar number
of students used no logical connective at all. Although the numbers
are small, the majority were in Year 8 and from high decile schools.
This is the group who, because of the closeness of their home language
environment to that of the school, were expected to be heavy users
of logical connectives, yet about one quarter failed to use any. |
|
|
|
Table
6.12. Distribution of logical connectives between two Premises.
|
|
|
Logical
Connectives |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
and |
26 |
22 |
23 |
25 |
16 |
14 |
18 |
48 |
and
then |
14 |
14 |
10 |
18 |
5 |
10 |
13 |
28 |
then |
5 |
9 |
5 |
9 |
4 |
4 |
6 |
14 |
so |
2 |
2 |
4 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
3 |
4 |
but |
2 |
3 |
0 |
5 |
1 |
2 |
2 |
5 |
or |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
No
LC |
10 |
9 |
10 |
9 |
4 |
5 |
10 |
19 |
|
|
|
As
had occurred in the Motorway task, the use of logical connectives
is fairly similar for the groups, with a few exceptions. More students
from high decile schools used logical connectives and a larger range
of them. They were also the group which chose most often not to
use a logical connective between Premises. This suggests that while
some students used more than one connective in their response, others
chose not to use any at all.
However, there
are differences in the use of some logical connectives in a Premise
- Consequence combination and a Premise - Premise combination. 'So'
as a causal connective is used more often to connect a Premise to
a Consequence, but almost always by students from high decile schools
and mostly by boys. The four students who used it to connect two
Premises together were all in Year 4 and, also, mainly students
from high decile schools. Peterson and McCabe (1991, p. 38), in
reviewing the literature, suggested that 'narrators use connectives
at transition points when they are departing from the timeline of
their narratives to insert other relevant information, specifically
so, but and occasionally because'. Below
are the four extracts in which so is used to join two Premises together.
and on Friday
they didn't bring any money in, so then another bank robber came
in and stole all their money again (Year 4 non-Pacific boy from
low decile school)
Q Five dollars,
and they leave it there, for five dollars they spend, so they
don't spend any money on Friday, and they spend five dollars more,
so it makes ten dollars (Year 4 boy from a high decile school)
on Monday
she had ten dollars, and um so on Tuesday and Wednesday she had
the same amount, in twenty dollars (Year 4 girl from a high decile
school)
on Monday
the man, the man went to his bank account and, he had .. ten dollars
in his bank account, so he, he, he left it to the next day to
get some money out (Year 4 girl from a high decile school)
As can be seen
from these extracts, none of the 'so's which connected two Premises
seemed to fulfil this pragmatic role. The second extract suggests
that the boy lost his thought pattern after a teacher utterance
and the 'so' marks the point where he picks up the thread of what
he is saying again. The other uses suggest that the speakers wanted
the following Premises to be Consequences but have not made the
connection sufficiently clear. McCabe and Peterson (1985), in research
on the use of connectives by different ages of children, also found
examples of 'so' where there were no causal links. Some of these
were later explained, while others remained unexplained. They found
no relationship to the age of speakers. On the other hand, Donaldson's
(1986) research into children's use of connectives including 'so',
found that logical causality was not mastered until the age of 8
and 9. Given that no similar examples were found amongst the Year
8 responses, it may be that part of the learning process for using
'so' effectively involves a period of time when it is used incorrectly.
This would also explain the incorrect use of 'because' in the Better
Buy task.
As well as logical
connectives, the students used a range of other linguistic devices
to suggest cohesion between ideas. One of these devices which was
used by many students was the use of an adverb such as 'still' or
'again', or an adjective such as 'another', or 'same', which was
used both as an adjective and as a noun. The following table outlines
those who used these devices. |
|
|
|
Table
6.13. Showing the distribution of devices to support the cohesion
of the text. |
|
|
Cohesion
devices |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total |
Boys |
Girls |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non-PI |
High |
 |
still |
0 |
7 |
4 |
3 |
3 |
1 |
3 |
7 |
again |
1 |
2 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
2 |
1 |
3 |
even
more |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
more |
7 |
7 |
7 |
7 |
1 |
3 |
10 |
14 |
comparatives |
5 |
6 |
8 |
3 |
1 |
3 |
7 |
11 |
superlatives |
6 |
0 |
3 |
3 |
4 |
2 |
0 |
6 |
another |
1 |
5 |
2 |
4 |
2 |
3 |
1 |
6 |
same |
11 |
8 |
7 |
12 |
7 |
4 |
8 |
19 |
|
|
|
There
are certainly significant differences between which groups using
these devices. Only boys used 'still' and only girls used superlatives,
such as 'shortest' and 'highest'. Students from high decile schools
were more likely to use 'more' and comparatives, such as ' bigger'
and 'lower', than students from low decile schools. Although the
number of examples was small, it would seem that students from low
decile schools were more likely to use superlatives than students
from high decile schools.
Compared to
the Better Buy task and to the Motorway task, where there were differences
between the text structures, it is interesting to see that there
are no trends for the different groups with this task. This could
be partially because the text structures themselves were limited
but maybe also because the students were uncertain about what was
expected of them in this task. With limited variety in text structures
and difficulty in determining what was an appropriate mathematical
response, there seemed little point in looking at the text structures
in comparison with appropriateness of response. Therefore, this
section has not been provided in this chapter. |
|
|
Hesitant
language |
The
Bank Account task was not readily understood by many students and
different groups employed different strategies for expressing uncertainty
in responding to it. |
|
|
|