Mathematical
explanations and justifications have an important role in mathematics
assessment (Bicknell, 1999). Yet, as was discussed in Chapter 1,
it would seem that there is limited knowledge about what constitutes
good examples of these. Part of the issue is knowing what the constituent
parts of explanations and justifications are and how the differences
between them are expressed. In this research, explanations were
considered to be the description of what was done in solving a problem;
and it had been expected that students would provide explanations
when responding to the Weigh Up and Motorway task. Justifications
were defined as the descriptions of why a certain strategy was adopted.
The Better Buy and Bank Account tasks were expected to illicit justifications.
Such a clear cut distinction was not found, especially when students
struggled with the purpose of the task, as was the case with Bank
Account.
There
was also a need to research the stages that students went through
as they learnt how to produce a mathematical explanation or justification.
Primary school children are not expected to produce mathematical
proofs, which have been described as 'perhaps the ultimate in justifications'
(Sowder & Harel, 1998, p. 670). Therefore, it was postulated that
there might be series of linguistic features that students would
need to learn as they progressed through school, so that, by the
time they reached university they had mastered all of the features
needed to construct a mathematical proof. It was anticipated that
research on the responses from these tasks might contribute to understanding
what these intermediate features might be. The differences found
between groups in how they used these features would suggest that
there is no clear pattern of mathematical language acquisition.
Instead, what language is acquired and valued within certain situations
is affected by a large number of issues. This chapter discusses
in detail differences between text structures and the issues which
seem to affect them, including the demographic characteristics of
the students and the task requirements.
First,
it is worth reviewing what was already known about explanations
and justifications. This research can be divided into what has been
done at the macro or micro level of analysis. At the macro level,
Krummheuer's (1995) work, based on the ideas of Toulmin (1969),
on the components on mathematical arguments has been used by several
other researchers, such as Yackel (2001) and Forman et al. (1998).
These components were: claims, which were the suggested solutions;
grounds, which were the unchallengeable facts from which the claims
were developed; warrants, which were the information joining the
grounds to the claims; and backings, which provided the constraints
under which the warrants were valid. These researchers used these
ideas to discover how students working together and with the teacher
developed 'collective argumentation' (Forman et al., 1998). They
were not looking so much at how individual students would produce
explanations. Other macro level analysis was undertaken by Sowder
and Harel (1998), which looked at high school and college students'
responses and classified them according to the evidence used in
them. These were: externally based proof schemes; empirical proof
schemes; analytic proof schemes. However, this macro level research
did not provide information on the linguistic structures which could
be related to these components of arguments or types of justifications.
The
micro level research on students' explanations was that done by
Bills (2002 and Bills & Grey, 2001). This research is considered
micro level as it documented the linguistic features which seemed
to be related to the accuracy of students' explanations as to 'what
was in their head' (Bills, 2002, p. 100). Students who used 'I'
or 'you' in a general sense, logical connectives such as 'because'
and 'if' and present tense were more likely to give an accurate
response. Typical classroom talk, such as using 'you', Bills (2002)
hypothesised, was more likely to be used when students were successful.
Certainly some of these linguistic features were evident in the
responses to the four tasks, particularly the use of logical connectives
and the use of personal pronouns. However, Bills did not relate
these linguistic features to the different components of the explanations.
By
identifying the different text elements and the typical ways in
which they were combined, we have been able to analyse the responses
at both the macro and micro level. This builds on the work of Hasan
(Halliday and Hasan,1985) who described how contextual configuration
could be used to predict the text structure. As well, the reverse
is also true, in that the text structure could be used to illuminate
the contextual configuration. Hasan's (Halliday and Hasan, 1985)
ideas about text structure require not only an identification of
the different text elements, but also the recognition of what elements
were compulsory, whether the positioning of elements were set and
whether elements could be reiterated. This then allows for an investigation
of linguistic features within the elements in order to attain a
better understanding of these elements and how they are placed together
to form the text structure. |
|
|
aContextual
configuration and text elements |
For
all of the tasks, the contextual configuration was that students
were in an interview situation at school with a teacher administrator
who was asking questions to ascertain their mathematical knowledge.
The particular knowledge being assessed was slightly different in
all cases but all required the students to provide insights on how
they perceived the mathematical relationship between objects, mathematical
or physical (numerical and quantitative comparisons). It was therefore
possible to see how the similarities in the text elements used across
tasks was related to the general situation in which the student
was operating. By looking at the differences between the responses
for the tasks, it was possible to see how the task specifications
as part of the contextual configuration influenced the text structures.
It was quite clear that no elements were used by every student in
every task. However, there were several elements which were used
by many students in more than one task. Their use across tasks suggested
that they were likely to be the text elements used consistently
by students in responding to any mathematical assessment task orally.
These elements were Premise, Consequence, Conclusion, Elaborator
and Supposition. Table 8.1 shows the number out of a total of 431
students who used each of these text elements. In this table, the
three sections of Weigh Up are considered different tasks, but there
were only 71 samples for the Explanation section of the Weigh Up
task. As the samples for the two 1997 tasks were of students from
low, middle and high decile schools and the two 2001 tasks had students
from only low and high decile schools there are problems in combining
these results. In order to give an overview of which elements were
used most often, the two groups of students, Pacific and non-Pacific,
were totalled together and this amount then halved. No amounts were
recorded for students from middle decile schools, and so amounts
in the low and high decile columns do not add up to the total amounts.
|
|
|
|
Table
8.1. Use of text elements by different groups. |
|
|
Text
Elements |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
introduction |
9 |
9 |
10 |
8 |
8 |
|
6 |
18 |
premise |
206 |
197 |
197 |
206 |
132 |
140 |
403 |
consequence |
127 |
130 |
117 |
140 |
74 |
99 |
257 |
physical
consequence |
48 |
43 |
45 |
46 |
32 |
30 |
91 |
conclusion |
46 |
49 |
38 |
57 |
22 |
41 |
95 |
supposition |
26 |
16 |
13 |
29 |
9 |
13 |
42 |
elaborator |
92 |
79 |
75 |
96 |
50 |
67 |
171 |
 |
|
|
|
This
table clearly shows that Premises were the most common text element
used, with Introductions being the least common. There were few
differences between groups using these elements. However, it would
appear that girls use Suppositions and Elaborators more often than
boys. It would also seem that Year 8 students tended to use more
elements than Year 4 students, although the amount of difference
varies for the different elements. Year 8 students used more Consequences,
Conclusions, Suppositions and Elaborators. Students from high decile
schools also used more Consequences, Conclusions and Elaborators
than their peers at low decile schools.
Within many
of these elements, there were variations. The next sections look
at each of these elements and their variations, before looking at
how they were combined. The following sections show that very rarely
did differences between groups persist across tasks, indicating
that the tasks as part of the contextual configuration had a significant
influence on the text structures. |
|
|
aIntroduction |
This
element was used by only a few students in responding to four tasks.
These were Motorway and the Weigh Up Plan, Description and Explanation.
Introductions set the scene for a personalised or generalised response
to the task. Examples of these Introductions were 'I went' or 'you
could'. Given that most of the scripts for the tasks have the teacher
administrators saying 'Tell me how' or 'Explain to me,' it is, perhaps,
more surprising to find that so few students began their responses
by acknowledging such a request with an Introduction. It would seem
that in some tasks a Premise - Elaborator combination was used by
many students to fulfil the same purpose. An example of this combination
would be 'You'd tell them to use one of these' which was said by
a Year 4 student in responding to the Explanation section of the
Weigh Up task. Table 8.2 sets out who used Introductions. |
|
|
|
Table
8.2. Use of Introductions in different tasks. |
|
|
Introductions |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
WU
Plan |
2 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
|
1 |
3 |
WU
Description |
2 |
1 |
3 |
0 |
3 |
0 |
3 |
WU
Explanation |
0 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
Motorway |
5 |
6 |
4 |
7 |
4 |
4 |
11 |
 |
|
|
|
Even
with such small numbers of students, there does not appear to be any
distinction between the groups who used Introductions. Motorway was
the only task where more than 10% of students used an Introduction.
|
|
|
aPremise |
In
explanations and justifications, there is a need to provide evidence
which would convince others (Sowder & Harel, 1998). The Premise is
the linguistic embodiment of the basis for this evidence and, therefore,
can be considered equivalent to Krummheuer's (1995) warrant. Certainly,
as can be seen in Table 8.1, Premises were used by most students in
most tasks, with little difference between groups. However, as can
be seen by the reduced number of low decile students who used Premises
in their responses to the Description part of the Weigh Up task, the
task requirements have an effect even on this fundamental component.
In these responses, Physical Consequences were used more often than
Premises, as students had to describe their physical actions. These
physical actions may have become the unspoken Premises. |
|
|
|
Table
8.3. Use of Premises in different tasks. |
|
|
Premises |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
Better
Buys |
36 |
36 |
36 |
36 |
24 |
|
24 |
72 |
WU
Plan |
25 |
31 |
31 |
35 |
22 |
23 |
66 |
WU
Description |
27 |
27 |
25 |
29 |
16 |
21 |
54 |
WU
Explanation |
36 |
34 |
34 |
36 |
23 |
24 |
70 |
Motorway |
36 |
36 |
36 |
36 |
24 |
24 |
72 |
Bank
Account |
36 |
33 |
35 |
34 |
23 |
24 |
69 |
|
|
|
Although
Krummheuer (1995) suggested that warrants were unchallengeable facts,
this was not the case with all Premises. Different types of Premises
reflected the type of evidence which was given and can, therefore,
be related to the proof schemes classified by Sowder and Harel (1998).
In the responses to the Better Buy task, there were hypothetical
or factual Premises. In the Motorway task responses, Premises were
divided into factual, mathematical and personal. For the Bank Account
task, there were personal, graphical or monetary Premises in the
responses. In the Weigh Up task, there were no clear distinctions
in the Premises which were used, as most students started their
responses by relating an action that they would do or had done to
one or more of the boxes.
Factual Premises
were in the responses to both the Better Buy and Motorway tasks
and used information given in the question or in a resource. In
the Better Buy task, factual Premises were more likely to be used
in inaccurate responses. Students who gave them were less likely
to combine them with Consequences and Conclusions to produce a train
of reasoning. On the whole, it was more likely that students who
gave factual Premises were girls, in Year 4 and attending low decile
schools.
Responses to
the Motorway task showed a similar relationship as only four students
followed a factual Premise with a Consequence whereas 13 students
followed a mathematical Premise with a Consequence. This would suggest
that students saw the most convincing evidence as deriving from
an external source, as was also the case with the externally based
proof schemes described by Sowder and Harel (1998). Only 4 out of
the 20 students, who used a factual Premise, gave an accurate answer
for this task. It seems that if students used this type of Premise,
they were unlikely to develop them into a logical reason. In this
task, the students who were most likely to give a factual Premise
were Year 4 girls from low decile schools. There did not seem to
be any differences in the ethnicity of the students who gave factual
responses.
As factual Premises
used the unchallengeable facts, from those given in the question
or in a resource used with the question, they could be considered
grounds for a mathematical argument. However, as they were less
likely to be used in an extended response, the links from them to
their corresponding claims were too tenuous for clear mathematical
explanations or justifications to be considered to have been given.
This was the case for the Year 4 Pacific girl who gave factual Premises
in the following response:
...Heaps
a go, heaps a cars go down the road every day and in nine minutes
.. more cars come in.
When the distribution
of students in Better Buy and Motorway who used factual Premises
is considered together, it would seem that Year 4 girls from low
decile schools were most likely to use these Premises.
Hypothetical
Premises were the other type of Premise used in responses to the
Better Buy task, but were more likely to be found in accurate responses.
These Premises proposed an example as the basis for their justification
and so could be classified as providing an empirical proof scheme
(Sowder & Harel, 1998). They began with the logical connective 'if'
and were most often followed by Consequences and then Conclusions,
so that logical trains of thought were developed. Of the 25 students
who used these Premises, 18 were in Year 8 but only 5 were from
low decile schools. There did not appear to be any difference due
to gender. In the Explanation section of the Weigh Up task, 25 students
also began their Premise with 'if', suggesting a similarity with
hypothetical Premises. Certainly, it would seem that in using 'if'
students marked that an example was being proposed as an aid towards
a general solution. It was interesting to note, however, that the
differences between the groups found in the responses to the Better
Buy task were not evident in the Weigh Up task. It is difficult
to know whether this was due to only having a sample size of 72
for each task, or whether the context drawn upon for each task affected
different groups' choice of linguistic features.
Mathematical
Premises in responses to the Motorway task were the only Premises
to be given in accurate responses. However, unlike the hypothetical
Premises, they did not have to be combined with a Consequence to
be considered an accurate and clear response. For example, six Year
8 students, four from Pacific communities, were able to provide
just a mathematical Premise and be considered correct. Given that
students 133 are asked to 'Explain to me how you got your answer',
providing a mathematical Premise such as 'Nine minutes times ninety-eight
cars' is acceptable. It gives all the necessary information to answer
the request appropriately. As mathematical Premises use mathematical
calculations as their evidence for their solutions, they have similarities
with factual Premises, as they draw on externally based proof schemes.
Out of the 42 students who gave a mathematical Premise, 29 were
Year 8 students. Although there would seem to be slightly fewer
Pacific students who gave mathematical Premises (9 out of 42 students),
there do not appear to be any gender or decile level of school attended
differences for non-Pacific students.
Mathematical
and hypothetical Premises were mostly used in accurate responses
and were usually combined with Consequences and Conclusions. In
considering them together, they appear to have been used mainly
by Year 8 students with no distinction according to gender. Distinctions
according to decile level of school attended occurred with hypothetical
Premises, but, in response to the Motorway task, the distinction
was between Pacific and non-Pacific students. However, there were
differences in the type of proof schemes produced. Hypothetical
Premises were used with Consequences and Conclusions to produce
empirical proof schemes, whilst mathematical Premises were developed
into externally-based proof schemes. It would seem that the task
requirements influenced the type of Premise necessary to achieve
an appropriate response. It would also seem that not all students
were able to recognise what was the most appropriate Premise to
use in responding to the task.
Personal Premises
were used in responses to both the Bank Account and Motorway tasks.
These Premises did not occur in accurate responses and did not link
to any kind of proof scheme. In the Motorway task, personal Premises
were most often mental processes such as 'guessed', 'think' and
'said'. In the Weigh Up task, 'think' was classified as a Supposition
and was often used to lessen the certainty of what was being proposed.
In responses to the Motorway task, personal Premises were more likely
to be used to express uncertainty about what they had to do. Only
one student combined a personal Premise with a Consequence. Students
who gave these Premises were most likely to be boys in Year 4, from
Pacific Island communities and attending low decile schools.
If these results
are considered with those for factual Premises, which also tended
to be used in inaccurate responses, it would seem that these types
of Premises were mostly used by Year 4 students. This probably reflects
the fact that these students did not have sufficient mathematics
to respond successfully to the tasks. Whereas girls tended to use
factual Premises which repeated information from the question, boys,
in responding to the Motorway task, tended instead to use a personal
Premise. It would be interesting to know whether teachers felt that
factual Premises showed that students knew more mathematics, as
they often included mathematical information from the question.
When examining responses for a similar NEMP mathematics task, Anthony
and Walshaw (2002) found that, when students repeated parts of the
question, these were accepted as explanations by the teacher administrators
as no further probing was done. This may mean that boys, in being
blunter about their lack of knowledge, may be perceived as knowing
less and thus being given more support to improve their mathematical
knowledge.
In responses
to the Bank Account task, personal Premises were clauses which had
a person, often unspecified, as the main actor. These personal Premises
were, therefore, of a different kind from those used in the Motorway
task. In responses to the Bank Account task, more students used
these Premises than any other kind, but more often than not they
were combined with other kinds of Premises within the one response.
There were no differences in the groups of students who gave this
Premise. Graphical and monetary Premises were also specific to responses
for the Bank Account task, as they related to the graph provided
as a resource or to money respectively. The responses to the Bank
Account task were the least complex and often were a series of personal,
graphical and monetary Premises. Sometimes, they were combined with
Consequences. As with personal Premises, there were no differences
in who gave these types of Premises.
Premises can
be considered the grounds for an argument, but were most likely
to be used in appropriate responses if they were hypothetical or
mathematical. It was less likely that, if a student used other types
of Premises, they would be combined with Consequences and Conclusions
into complex explanations and justifications. This was because the
evidence which they provided needed to be a strong foundation from
which a logical train of thought could be developed. Although it
would have seemed that factual Premises based on information supplied
in the question or in a resource would also have provided this basis,
these Premises were rarely combined with Consequences and Conclusions
and used in appropriate responses. |
|
|
aConsequences |
If
Premises of a particular type can be considered grounds, then Consequences
have the potential to match Krummheuer's (1995) warrants and backings,
as they could contain the information that relates the grounds to
the claims. In order to produce an acceptable mathematical explanations
and justifications, it is necessary for the grounds to be manipulated
in some way, so that the logical relationship to the claims is clarified.
This includes identifying the conditions under which the manipulation
is valid. Esty (1992) discussed the use of logical connectives in
identifying the conditions for which certain mathematical statements
were true. In a general sense, Consequences can be considered warrants
and the way they are connected to their corresponding Premises can
be considered the backings. The conditions for when a manipulation
is valid is often contained within the logical connectives joining
the Premises to the Consequences, as well as within Elaborators
connected to Consequences. In this section, the logical connectives
used in a Premise - Consequence and Consequence - Consequence are
also discussed.
Consequences
were of two kinds; logical and Physical. Logical Consequences always
described the result of a manipulation of Premises and were never
used by themselves. Table 8.4 outlines who used logical Consequences
in their responses to the various tasks. |
|
|
|
Table
8.4. Use of Consequences in different tasks. |
|
|
Consequences |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
Better
Buys |
18 |
20 |
10 |
28 |
9 |
|
13 |
38 |
WU
Plan |
20 |
26 |
24 |
22 |
13 |
17 |
46 |
WU
Description |
25 |
25 |
22 |
28 |
15 |
19 |
50 |
WU
Explanation |
35 |
31 |
32 |
34 |
21 |
23 |
66 |
Motorway |
15 |
14 |
13 |
16 |
7 |
16 |
29 |
Bank
Account |
14 |
14 |
16 |
12 |
9 |
11 |
28 |
|
|
|
If
Consequences are considered to be warrants, then it is quite clear
that students felt that the explanations for some tasks required
them more than others. As students moved through the Weigh Up task,
more began to use Consequences so that when they gave their final
explanation only five students did not use a Consequence. In regard
to the different groups, there appears to be no difference in use
according to gender. On the whole, Year 8 students used more Consequences
than their Year 4 peers, but most of this difference is accounted
for in the responses to the Better Buy task. More students attending
high decile schools also used Consequences, but there were no very
large differences for any particular task.
In almost all
tasks, Consequences combined with certain Premises were linked to
responses which were considered accurate and clear. For example,
in the Better Buy task, responses which combined hypothetical Premises
with Consequences and Conclusions were often accurate. It was more
than likely that, in responding to this task, it was a Year 8 student
not attending a low decile school who used Consequences. As well,
in responses to the Motorway task, there was no difference according
to gender in who used Consequences, but there was also little difference
between Year 4 and Year 8 students. As was seen in Table 5.6, although
students attending high decile schools were slightly more likely
to use Consequences than non- Pacific students, both groups were
far more likely to use them than Pacific students attending low
decile schools. In the Weigh Up task, there were slightly more boys
who used a Consequence in their response to the Plan section. However,
there were no other differences between groups using Consequences
in this section. In the Description section, slightly more Year
8 students used Consequences than Year 4 students. In the responses
to this section of the task, there were no differences in gender,
ethnicity or school decile level. In the responses to the Explanation
section of the task, there were no differences between groups who
used Consequences in their responses.
The distribution
of logical connectives between Premises and Consequences and between
Consequences suggested that the task requirements influenced their
use. When a Premise began with an 'if', as was the case for hypothetical
Premises, it was unlikely that another connective would be provided
in front of the following Consequence. However, in long responses,
such as those given for the different sections of the Weigh Up task
where Premise - Consequence combinations were iterated, some students
would often use several different logical connectives to join Premises
and Consequences and groups of Consequences together. On the whole,
it was more common for students to use narrative rather than causal
connectives. However, some tasks, such as Motorway, seemed to encourage
students to use more causal connectives than others. This may be
because the questions in this task asked more explicitly for an
explanation than the Bank Account task did. Those who were more
likely to use logical connectives also seemed to change depending
upon the task. When all the connectives are considered together,
it was more likely that the students who used them would be in Year
8 and more probably not be attending a low decile school or not
be a Pacific Islander. However, these distinctions were not always
clear cut across tasks, with more Year 4 students using more logical
connectives when responding to the Plan section of the Weigh Up
task.
Physical Consequences
were the other type of Consequences. They could be found without
Premises and were used mainly in responses to the Plan and the Description
sections of the Weigh Up task. They described the result of a physical
manipulation of the boxes and so the action itself can be considered
as a silent Premise. |
|
|
|
Table
8.5. Use of Physical Consequences in different tasks. |
|
|
Physical
Consequences |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Middle |
High |
 |
WU
Plan |
11 |
10 |
11 |
10 |
10 |
4 |
7 |
21 |
WU
Description |
31 |
32 |
31 |
32 |
20 |
21 |
22 |
63 |
WU
Explanation |
6 |
1 |
3 |
4 |
2 |
4 |
1 |
7 |
|
|
|
As
Table 8.5 shows, there was little difference in those who used Physical
Consequences in the three sections of the task. As it was the same
students who were giving explanations in all three sections, it
is quite clear that the task requirements had a strong influence
on the inclusion of these text elements. The Description section
of this task required students to talk about what they were doing
as they were doing it. In a shared situation where the students
were physically manipulating the boxes, it is not surprising to
find that Physical Consequences were the most common elements in
responses.
Sometimes Physical
Consequences were followed by logical Consequences. In responses
to the Plan section of the task, 21 students gave a Physical Consequence,
but only three followed it with a logical Consequence. However,
the situation changed in the Description part of the task. Of the
63 students who used Physical Consequences, 36 followed them with
logical Consequences. 94% of accurate and clear responses to the
Description part of the Weigh Up task contained Physical Consequences
combined with logical Consequences. A considerable number used causal
connectives between them, with 25 students using 'so' and a further
three students using 'so then'. More causal connectives were used
in this position than in any other connections between text elements.
These causal connectives were most likely to be used by girls, Year
8 students and least likely by students attending low decile schools.
Although fewer students attending low decile schools had used logical
Consequences, the difference in causal connectives is larger.
Consequences
are important features in mathematical explanations and justifications,
as they fill in a step from relating the information contained in
the Premise to the actual claim being made. As such, they can be
seen as fulling the role of warrants. However, in joining logical
Consequences to Premises, it would seem that if a causal connective
was used, it would be 'if' and would occur before the Premise. On
the 137 whole, narrative connectives such as 'and' were most often
used between Premises and logical Consequences. When Physical Consequences
were used in responses to the Description part of the task, a large
number of students used the causal connective 'so' to join them
to logical Consequences. Research with responses to other tasks
which require physical manipulation of materials needs to be done
to see whether the use of causal connectives is more likely in this
situation. Although Consequences could be considered equivalent
to warrants, when considering how backings are linguistically represented
the use of narrative rather than causal connectives limits the information
about when the warrants were valid. Backings are also linguistically
represented through Elaborators, as they also provided extra information
about Consequences. |
|
|
aConclusions |
Conclusions
were text elements which were not found by themselves. On the whole
they rounded off an explanation, often coming after a Premise and
a Consequence. They are, in many ways the linguistic equivalent of
Krummheuer's (1995) claims, as they are the outcome of the explanation
or justification. As can be seen in Table 8.6, they were used in responses
to all tasks, although it was only in the responses to Better Buy
that more than half the students used Conclusions. This is not as
surprising as it first appears. The scripts for Better Buy and Motorway
both initially ask students for a result of a calculation before going
on to ask for a justification and an explanation, respectively. Therefore,
a repetition of this result is not necessary for the explanation to
make sense. Yet, in explaining why one box is better value for money
than the other, it is not unreasonable to include a second mention
of the actual result. As well, some students pre-empted the second
question in this task and so the Conclusion came at the beginning
of the explanation as well as at the end. |
|
|
|
Table
8.6. Use of Conclusions in different tasks. |
|
|
Consequences |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
Better
Buys |
19 |
25 |
15 |
29 |
8 |
|
18 |
44 |
WU
Plan |
0 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
2 |
2 |
WU
Description |
2 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
3 |
WU
Explanation |
16 |
10 |
11 |
15 |
8 |
9 |
26 |
Motorway |
7 |
10 |
8 |
9 |
4 |
10 |
17 |
Bank
Account |
2 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
3 |
|
|
|
In
the Bank Account task, there was no common way for completing the
story. Three students finished their responses with 'that's all',
which, like all Conclusions, signalled the end, although no reference
was made to the requirements of the task. The responses to each
of the sections of the Weigh Up task were varied, as there were
many appropriate ways to explain how to determine the order of boxes
from heaviest to lightest. Given that this was so, it is fascinating
to see that over a third of students provided a Conclusion in their
responses to the Explanation section. It may well be that the imaginary
nature of the last part of this task - 'explain it to some one else
in your class' - and the fact that students were giving their third
version of this explanation encouraged more students to complete
their responses with Conclusions.
When considering
the results in Table 8.6, there appear to be few differences according
to gender. In the Better Buy task, slightly more boys used Conclusions
than girls, but in the Explanation section of the Weigh Up task,
slightly more girls used Conclusions than boys. It would seem that
Year 8 students and students at high decile schools are much more
likely to use Conclusions than other groups. The large number of
students who included Conclusions in their responses to the Better
Buy task have had a significant effect on these results.
It was also
in responses to the Better Buy task that the two types of Conclusions
were seen most clearly. These were implicit and explicit Conclusions.
In responses to the Better Buy task, explicit Conclusions were ones
which made reference to better value for money, which was what the
original question had asked about. An implicit Conclusion did not
make direct reference to better value, but instead made a veiled
reference through using words such as 'only' or 'cheaper'. In the
responses to the Better Buy task, boys and Year 8 students and students
from high decile schools were more likely to use explicit Conclusions,
whilst implicit Conclusions were used by equivalent numbers of boys
and girls, but also by more Year 8 students and students attending
high decile schools. More students used implicit Conclusions than
explicit ones in this task.
In the remaining
task, it was not always easy to distinguish between these two types
of Conclusions. However, on the whole, more implicit Conclusions
were used than explicit Conclusions. This was the case in the responses
to all three parts of the Weigh Up task as can be seen in Tables
4.2, 4.15 and 4.28. In these responses, students were far more likely
to mention the boxes being 'in order', which was coded as implicit,
than to say something about the boxes being 'in order from lightest
to heaviest', which would have been coded as an explicit Conclusion.
Logical connectives
were also important in joining Consequences to Conclusions in responses
to only some of the tasks. Given that extra information for the
backing for the claims in the Conclusions could be provided in these
logical connectives, it is surprising to not find them in this position
in responses to all tasks. In responses to the Better Buy task,
though 'and' was still the most often used connective, 6 students
used 'so' and a further 5 used 'but'. These causal connectives were
more likely to be used by Year 8 students (as also were the narrative
connectives in this position) and least likely by students attending
low decile schools. In the responses to the Plan and Description
parts of the Weigh Up task, very few connectives were used between
Consequences and Conclusions. In the responses to the Explanation
section, 23 students used connectives, but only seven of these were
causal. As had been the case with logical connectives surrounding
Consequences, more girls than boys used narrative connectives. In
the Motorway task, only 7 students used connectives before the Conclusion
and, of these, none were causal. There were not enough Conclusions
in the Bank Account task for logical connectives to be worth considering.
It would seem
that Conclusions were an optional element for many students when
giving mathematical explanations and justifications. However, for
some tasks, like Better Buy, the majority of students felt that
their responses should include a Conclusion of some sort. When Conclusions
were given, they were usually implicit, with the listener left to
make the final jump between the speaker's response and the actual
question asked. As backings are considered to be the information
which defined when the claim is true, it was felt that the logical
connectives would be the linguistic embodiment of these backings,
particularly around Consequences. As Conclusions represented the
claims themselves, it was also felt that the logical connectives
around them could also constitute part of this backing. However,
on the whole very few connectives were used and mostly these were
narrative rather than causal. This suggests that it was uncommon
for students in primary schools to use causal connectives to identify
the conditions under which claims were valid. |
|
|
aSuppositions |
Suppositions
could not be related to any of the components of explanations described
by Krummheuer (1997). Instead, this text element was used to add
a propositional element. Suppositions were clauses which made preceding
or following clauses propositional or more vague. As a result, they
never came at the beginning or the end of an explanation. Although
Suppositions were only coded within the Weigh Up task, they were
also evident in responses to the Bank Account and Motorway tasks.
However, they were considered as hedges and are described in the
sections on hesitant language, at the end of the chapters on these
tasks.
In the Weigh
Up task, students needed to talk about courses of action that they
may take rather than those that they had taken. Using Suppositions
gave students ways to discuss these possible courses of action.
This enabled students to give the generic responses described by
Bills and Grey (2001) which used a specific example to illustrate
a general case. This is the situation in 'say if C was the heaviest
you stick that, that one down at the bottom'. 'Say' was coded as
a supposition, as it situates what follows as a possibility rather
than a definite action. |
|
|
|
Table
8.7. Use of Suppositions in different tasks. |
|
|
Suppositions |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
Middle |
High |
 |
WU
Plan |
10 |
5 |
3 |
12 |
6 |
6 |
3 |
15 |
WU
Description |
9 |
5 |
6 |
8 |
1 |
8 |
5 |
14 |
WU
Explanation |
7 |
6 |
4 |
9 |
2 |
6 |
5 |
13 |
|
|
|
Table
8.7 shows that there were very few distinctions across tasks. This
is surprising, as the Description part of this task required the
students to describe what they were doing rather than what they
would be doing. It was not expected that there would be uncertainty
in this. However, in responses to this part of the task, the Suppositions
used were predominantly 'I think' which lessens the certainty of
what was proposed, as was the case in 'I think that one there's
the second heaviest'. These did not set up a proposition in the
same way as 'say' did.
There were some
differences in the groups who used Suppositions as girls and Year
8 students used them more than other groups. It would also seem
that students attending middle decile schools were slightly more
likely to use them than students attending other decile levels of
schools.
Suppositions
added uncertainty to information contained in other text elements.
Sometimes they were used by students to lessen a possible loss of
face, whilst at other 140 times they were used to show that an action
was proposed rather than actually happening. |
|
|
aElaborators |
Elaborators
like Suppositions were not used in responses to all tasks. They
added further information to other text elements and could be considered
as contributing to backings given in mathematical arguments as sometimes
the extra information was about the conditions for when the claim
was valid. This can be seen in the following example: 'test for
the lightest one and then put the two left overs in the middle whichever's
the heaviest obviously goes that way'. 'Whichever's the heaviest'
is the Elaborator and provides information which constrains what
'goes that way'.
Elaborators
were combined with all other text elements but they were most likely
to be combined with Premises and Consequences, which were the two
most commonly used text elements. Table 8.8 provides information
on who used Elaborators in which task. |
|
|
|
Table
8.8. Use of Elaborators in different tasks. |
|
|
Elaborators |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
WU
Plan |
25 |
21 |
18 |
28 |
28 |
|
14 |
46 |
WU
Description |
18 |
20 |
17 |
21 |
21 |
11 |
38 |
WU
Explanation |
34 |
30 |
30 |
34 |
34 |
18 |
64 |
Bank
Account |
15 |
8 |
10 |
13 |
13 |
7 |
23 |
|
|
|
Unlike
Suppositions, there do seem to be task differences in the number
of students who used Elaborators. 64 out of a possible 71 students
used Elaborators in the Explanation section of the Weigh Up task.
As this section produced the longest, most complex responses, it
would seem that Elaborators contributed to this complexity. However,
the responses to the Bank Account task were also long but often
the text structures were quite simple compared to those found in
the responses to this section of the Weigh Up task. Only 23 students
used Elaborators in the Bank Account task and these were mostly
used with Premises, as can be seen in the following two tables.
Tables 8.9 and
8.10 show how Elaborators were distributed with Premises and Consequences.
Although some Introductions, Physical Consequences and Conclusions
included Elaborators, there were so few of these that they have
not been included. |
|
|
|
Table
8.9. Use of Elaborators with Premises. |
|
|
|
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
WU
Plan |
12/35 |
6/31 |
7/31 |
11/35 |
8/22 |
|
5/23 |
18/66 |
WU
Description |
9/27 |
5/27 |
6/25 |
7/29 |
5/16 |
4/21 |
14/54 |
WU
Explanation |
24/36 |
21/34 |
18/34 |
27/36 |
16/23 |
13/24 |
45/70 |
Bank
Account |
12/36 |
3/36 |
8/36 |
10/36 |
6/24 |
7/24 |
18/72 |
|
|
|
Table
8.9 shows that only in responses to the Explanation section of the
Weigh Up task were Elaborators combined with Premises by the majority
of students. In the other tasks, about a quarter of students combined
Premises with Elaborators. When all the results are considered together,
there seems to be little difference between Year 4 and 8 students
or students attending different decile levels of schools. Although
Premises were used equally by boys and girls, girls were more likely
to combine them with Elaborators. |
|
|
|
Table
8.10. Use of Elaborators with Consequences. |
|
|
|
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low |
|
High |
 |
WU
Plan |
18/20 |
17/26 |
19/24 |
16/22 |
10/13 |
|
14/17 |
35/46 |
WU
Description |
12/25 |
14/25 |
13/22 |
13/28 |
9/15 |
13/19 |
26/50 |
WU
Explanation |
29/34 |
25/30 |
25/30 |
29/34 |
18/18 |
20/24 |
25/64 |
Bank
Account |
6/17 |
3/17 |
5/19 |
4/15 |
2/11 |
6/13 |
9/34 |
|
|
|
As
was noted in the Weigh Up chapter, a greater proportion of Consequences
were combined with Elaborators than Premises. In the responses to
the Bank Account task, this was not the case. Slightly more girls
than boys used Elaborators with Consequences but the distinction
is quite small. There does not seem to be any distinction between
age level or decile level of school attended. The results of Tables
4.14, 4.27 and 4.41 suggest that there is no clear relationship
between using an elaborator with a Consequence and having a response
considered clear and accurate.
Elaborators
were text elements which only appeared in students' responses when
combined with another text element. Their role was to provide further
information than was already given in the text element with which
they were combined. Proportionally, they were used with Consequences
more often than any other text elements. When Elaborators were combined
with Consequences, they could also provide information about when
the warrants given in the Consequences were valid and thus could
be considered as part of the backings. |
|
|
aCombining
text elements |
The
distribution of text elements throughout students' responses to
the four mathematics assessment tasks was not random. Although there
were always some exceptions, on the whole, Introductions began the
responses, Premises preceded Consequences which preceded Conclusions.
Suppositions and Elaborators were always combined with other elements.
Premises, Consequences and Premise - Consequence combinations (with
their corresponding Elaborators and Suppositions) could be iterated
several times throughout a response. Figure 8.1 provides a flowchart
of the possible decision making process that a student goes through
subconsciously in order to provide a response to mathematical assessment
tasks.
This flowchart
is idealised in two ways. The first is that it is unlikely that
even unconsciously students would go through such a decision making
process. The other way in which it is idealised is that the sort
of response that such a flowchart would produce would match very
few of the actual examples that were given. This is because there
was so much variation in the responses. Some of this variation could
be accounted
for in regard to the accuracy of the responses, as certain combinations
such as Premise - Consequence or Physical Consequence - Consequence
combinations were more likely to be used in accurate responses.
However, there were differences in the likelihood of these combinations
appearing in accurate responses, depending on which task students
were responding to. This suggests that there was more to knowing
how to provide appropriate mathematical explanations and justifications
than knowing how to combine text elements. This is discussed further
in the next section. |
|
|
 |
Figure
8.1: Flowchart showing steps to forming a mathematical explanation. |
|
|
aContextual
Configuration |
It was suggested
in Figure 1.1, that in order to be assessed as accurate in responding
to mathematical tasks, students need to have knowledge of mathematics
and mathematical language. This research suggests that what is
required is far more complex. Although most groups of students
do seem to have appropriate text elements in their linguistic
repertoire, these were not always used in a mathematically appropriate
way. Bills (2002) had also found that, although students had certain
linguistic features when discussing non-mathematical topics, they
did not always chose to use them when discussing mathematical
topics. Bills (2002) suggested that, when students were accurate,
they were more likely to use certain features such as logical
connectives. However, our research suggests that there were some
students who provided accurate answers in tasks such as Motorway,
but were unable to provide accurate descriptions of their thinking
processes. This can be seen in the following example:
Q
Is it an estimation?
Q
.........(counting to herself)......one hundred and, oh .. umm,
nine hundred and ninety.
Q
Because each car goes, every 98 cars every minute so then make
it so it's, then you go to one hundred and eighty, and then one
hundred and ninety and one hundred and eighty, you go until you
get to ninety something (voice trailing off at the end).
In trying
to understand the variety of students' responses, the relationship
between the contextual configuration and the text structure needs
further examination. In cases such as shop encounters, Hasan (Halliday
& Hasan, 1985) suggested that there is a clear relationship between
the contextual configuration and the text structure. The compulsory
text elements such as the buyer's description of what they want
comes at a set place in the interaction, along with optional elements
such as the vendor's enquiry about the buyer's needs. The contextual
configuration was such that the roles of vendor and buyer are
clearly delineated by the event of buying something, which restricts
significantly what is appropriate for each participant to say.
Krummheuer's (1997) description of the components of mathematical
arguments suggested that certain features would be evident in
mathematical explanations and justifications. This would thus
also provide a clear relationship between the contextual configuration
and the text structure. It would seem that Premises, Consequences,
Conclusions and Elaborators can be related to grounds, warrants,
claims and backings at least in a loose way. It would also seem,
that in giving responses to mathematics assessment tasks, primary
school children draw upon this set of text elements. However,
not all of these text elements would be specific to mathematical
assessments. Nor were some text elements and combinations likely
to provide appropriate mathematical responses. For example, students
who used personal or factual Premises were unlikely to be giving
accurate responses.
So what influenced
students to give appropriate responses? There is a relationship
between the contextual configuration and text structure and this
can provide information on the stage that these students were
at in giving appropriate mathematical responses. In order to act
appropriately in these situations, students 144 needed first to
recognise the contextual configuration that they were in and then
draw upon three interconnected pieces of knowledge: appropriate
mathematical knowledge; knowledge of how to interact with a teacher;
and knowledge of how to structure mathematical explanations. These
can be seen in Figure 8.2.
|
|
|
 |
Figure
8.2: Knowledge drawn upon to produce an appropriate response. |
|
|
If students did not recognise that there was a need to show their
mathematical knowledge in the interaction, then it is unlikely that
students drew upon their knowledge of mathematics or the structure
of mathematical explanations (as discussed in the previous chapter
on story telling). This can be seen in the following response from
the Better Buy task by a Year 4 girl from a low decile school:
Because you
couldn't, it's easy if you had two kids you could get them to
share two, and a half each, because if they had this half, one
and a half each they'd complain that one was bigger and like that,
one was bigger and one was smaller.
This girl used
many of the features of mathematical explanations such as a Premise
- Consequence combination joined with causal connectives. However,
the mistaken belief that the task required a description of a shopping
strategy means that the response cannot be considered appropriate,
as it did not show what she knew mathematically.
If students
recognised the purpose of the interaction as one requiring them
to show what they knew mathematically, they still needed to combine
their understanding in the other three areas to produce an appropriate
response. The responses documented in this report suggest that it
is not often a lack of knowledge of how to interact appropriately
with a teacher which inhibited students' production of mathematical
responses. Yet there were instances where students stated that they
did not know the answer or refused to answer altogether, which could
be seen as a breaking of the pragmatic rules of conversation. In
the responses to the Bank Account task, of the 12 students who gave
these sorts of responses, only one was from a high decile school.
This task was difficult for all students, so it was interesting
to see that these were not considered valid responses by students
attending high decile schools.
Often, these
sort of responses could be seen as occurring foremost because of
the students' lack of mathematical knowledge. However, this was
not always the case as can be seen in the following interaction
between the teacher administrator and a Year 8 boy from a high decile
school:
Q
.....I don't know.
Q
Nine hundred.
Q
Um, because it says nine minutes so I just went up to nine hundred.
Although his
initial response is that he did not know, when questioned further
by the teacher administrator, he was quite capable of doing the
mathematics. It is hard to know why he chose not to show what he
was capable of initially. Maybe aspects of the situation such as
receiving no feedback on the accuracy of his performance confused
him about what was required from him by the teacher administrator.
It may be that in the classroom in which he had learnt about acceptable
responses in similar situations, that confessions of ignorance were
acceptable to his teacher. They, therefore, did not break the pragmatic
rule of providing a response which fitted the hearer's expectations.
Conversely, it may be that, although he knew what was required of
him, he did not share the teacher's belief that it was necessary
for him to do this. In this case, although the teacher administrator
may believe the contextual configuration is fixed, the student feel
that other options, such as non-compliance are available.
The third area
of knowledge that students need in order to provide an appropriate
mathematical explanation or justification was knowledge of the appropriate
text structures. The responses examined in this report suggest that
acceptable responses for this age group, although drawn from a restricted
set of text elements, can be extremely variable. As has been reported
in each chapter, there are some text combinations, such as Premise
- Consequence, which seem to be correlated with accurate and clear
responses. However, in the previous example, this boy did not explain
how he gained this answer clearly, even though he uses a Premise
- Consequence combination joined with a causal connective. It may
be that the range of proof schemes which are appropriate in primary
school in responding to these different tasks encourages such diversity
in text structures, even when students have sufficient mathematical
knowledge. At later stages in their school careers, it may be that
they are restricted to analytical proof schemes which Sowder and
Harel (1998) described as 'the ultimate in justifications' and thus
the text structures are also restricted. Further investigation needs
to occur to determine the validity of this conjecture.
When students'
responses are considered holistically, it can be seen that more
than just mathematical understanding and being able to structure
a mathematical response is required. This includes having an awareness
of what is involved in the contextual configuration and accepting
it as valid. Once students have recognised and agreed on the need
to show their mathematical knowledge, they then need to access and
combine the appropriate mathematical knowledge, ways of interacting
with a teacher administrator and knowledge of text structures of
mathematical explanations and justifications. |
|
|
aConclusion |
In the responses to the different mathematical tasks, students combined
a series of text elements (Introductions, Premises, Consequences,
Conclusions, Suppositions and Elaborators) into text structures.
Many of these text elements could be related to the 146 constituent
parts that Krummheuer (1997) proposed for a mathematical argument.
It was also possible to see a relationship between the type of Premise
provided and the proof schemes described by Sowder and Harel (1998).
This suggested that the contextual configuration had an effect on
the text structures needed for an appropriate mathematical explanation
and justification. The flowchart given in Figure 8.1 suggested a
series of decision making points that students went through in producing
their responses to the different assessment tasks.
However, it
was also clear that there was immense variability in the responses,
so it may not just be mathematical or language ability alone which
determines whether a student produced an appropriate mathematical
response. It would seem that not only should the contextual configuration
be recognised by both participants - the student and the teacher
administrator - but also it must be accepted by the student as valid
for them to then draw upon their mathematical knowledge, language
knowledge and interactional knowledge. |
|