aTask
and acceptable answers |
This
task came from the 1997 administration and asked students to indicate
which of two boxes of Pebbles was better value for money. Results
were selected from 6 boys and 6 girls of high, middle and low decile
schools. There was no special Pasifika group in 1997.
Both Year 4
and Year 8 students were asked the first three questions. Year 8
students were asked the additional questions numbered 4 - 6. |
|
Place
the 100g and 50 g boxes of Pebbles in front
of the student. |
In
this activity you will be using some boxes of Pebbles.
The big box holds 100 grams of Pebbles and
costs $1.30. The smaller box holds 50 grams of Pebbles
and costs 60 cents. |
1. |
Which one is better value for money? |
|
Prompt:
Which box would give you more Pebbles for the money? |
2. |
Why is that box better value for money? |
3. |
How do you know that? |
|
|
|
Year
8 only |
Place
the 20g box of Pebbles in front of the student. |
4.
|
This
box costs 30 cents. Which is the better buy - this 20g
box or this 100g? |
Point
to the 20 g box. |
5. |
If I wanted 100 g of Pebbles, how many of these boxes
would I need? |
6. |
How did you work that out? |
|
|
|
Figure
3.1 Instructions for the Better Buy question. |
|
|
|
|
|
We
chose to compare responses on the first three questions that were
given to both age groups. Logically, this common task requires a
comparison of two dimensions, weight or mass and price, and three
mathematical calculations: 50g+50g=100g, 60c+60c=$1.20, and $1.20
< $1.30. This required the students to combine two values, price
and weight, and select the one with the better ratio. Asking for
better value implies selecting less cost for the same amount, or
same cost for a greater amount.
The major difference
between the subgroups that were asked this question was that only
7 of 36 Year 4 students were accurate in their reasoning for why
one box was better value for money, and 27 of 36 of Year 8 students
could give appropriate answers. More than half of the Year 4 students
attended to only one dimension, usually price, and several of those
who noted both dimensions either did not compare them or were inaccurate
in the necessary calculation. Some indicated that they did not believe
that value for money depended on a comparison, one saying 'because
they 21 probably wouldn't cheat on you if they were good' (Year
4 girl from a middle decile school).
Table 3.1 below
shows the focus of the answers given by Year 4 students. In addition
to the 21 (48%) who attended to only one element in the problem,
there were 9 students who attended to both but did not have a suitable
way of making a direct comparison, such as doubling the price of
the 50 gram box and comparing that price to the price of the larger
box which held twice the weight. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
An
answer that incorporated both weight and price but did not integrate
them adequately was:
Because it's
less in money and um, like if you get them for kids to eat this
is much small, this is less (Year 4 girl from a low decile school)
The concept
of value for money as the result of a calculation appears to be
difficult for this age group and particularly for middle and low
decile students.
Both successful
and unsuccessful students came from schools in all decile ranges.
However, of those who were successful a higher proportion of students
came from high decile schools than middle or low decile schools.
The concept was considerably more familiar to students in Year 8,
as shown in Table 3.2 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
In
this age range, 75% of the students gave an appropriate answer.
The next highest percentage of students was those who identified
both cost and size as important but did not explain clearly what
gave value for money. Although correct students came from schools
in all deciles, all students who attended to only one element were
from 22 low decile schools and most of students who mentioned both
elements but did not clearly express value for money were from middle
or low decile schools. It appeared that both cognitive and linguistic
issues contributed to this.
A good example
of an answer that did give value for money was:
Fifty grams…
um this one [Q] Because if you times that by two it’s a
hundred grams and, um, sixty plus sixty is a hundred and twenty,
so that’s a dollar twenty and selling that for a dollar
thirty. (Year 8 girl from a middle decile school)
|
|
|
|
|
|
aLinguistic
elements employed |
Comparison
of mathematical agents and processes to non-mathematical agents and
processes |
Agents |
As
there were significant differences in the length of each response,
it was not useful to look simply at the number of each expression
in each category. Instead, it was decided to use a ratio of the number
of mathematical terms compared to everyday terms. In the category
of agent, we compared the number of times that students used the Pebble
boxes (or a pronoun for them) as the main agent of a clause compared
to their use of personal pronouns (including elliptical ones, as in
the case of imperatives) (see Halliday, 1985). This is because, in
the mathematics register, how things are named is important. In most
mathematics, mathematical objects are the doers of actions rather
than people. It was thought, therefore, that concentration on the
boxes of Pebbles more closely resembled the mathematics register than
the students describing themselves or others as the doers of the actions.
The overall analysis is summarised in Tables 3.3 and 3.4. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The
largest differences in use of linguistic structures were between
Year 4 and Year 8 students, although there were also marked differences
by gender.
Because of the
size of the groups analysed (only 6 students per cell), apparently
large differences between the small subgroups could be caused by
one or two students who had different response patterns. This was
the case for the ratio of middle decile Year 4 students using a
mathematical object in their clauses, in comparison to low and high
decile groups.
Year 4 students
used a mathematical object as an agent nine times as often as they
used a person as an agent. This use was usually a reference to 'this
box', 'that', or 'it'. When only two boxes were being compared this
was all the reference that was required for clarity. There were
few references to 'the bigger box' while most were to 'that one'
or 'it'. Year 8 students also used a high proportion of references
to the boxes as an agent using these terms, but were more likely
to use a personal pronoun as the agent of their clauses.
The ratio of
mathematical agent to personal pronoun as agent shows an irregular
pattern across deciles, with middle decile students using a lower
proportion of personal pronouns than the low and high decile students.
This may have been because the middle decile students said less
in response to this question than did the low and high decile students.
This one because
if you've got, you can get two of these for a lower price of that
and you get the same amount, you can buy two of these you get
a hundred grams and it's only a dollar twenty. Q Because that
you just, because if you bought two you'd times that by two, sixty
cents, it's a dollar twenty and net fifty net grams by two you
get a hundred gram net (Year 8 boy attending a high decile school)
The older students
who were more likely to use mathematical calculations were much
more likely than the younger ones to use a number or a mathematical
operation as the agent of a clause than were the younger students.
As was discussed
in Chapter 1, Bills and Grey (2001) showed that students who used
'I' and 'you' in their responses were more likely to give accurate
answers especially if they were also categorised as 'generic' or
'general'. This was because they were not referring specifically
to themselves or the teacher administrator but rather to the general
actions that any person would do. Given that Year 8 students were
more likely to provide an accurate response and use 'you' in their
responses, it seemed that a similar relationship was evident in
our data. Bills and Grey did suggest that 'you' was common classroom
talk and so it was students who were comfortable with the mathematics
who were more likely to use classroom ways of discussing mathematics.
However, more investigation into this was needed. This is discussed
further in the next section on Text Structures. |
|
|
|
|
|
Process
words |
|
|
In
the processes, the ratio was one between mathematical and non-mathematical
verbs. Mathematical verbs showed a relationship between things or
doing mathematical actions such as 'measuring', 'estimating' and
quasi-mathematical actions such as 'figuring out'. Non-mathematical
verbs described other types of actions such as 'putting on', 'taking
off' or personal actions such as 'saying', 'thinking', 'having'
or static verbs such as 'is' in 'this is a box'. These final verbs
need to be distinguished from the relationship verbs such as 'this
is heavier' where the 'is' is describing a relationship and is very
typical of the mathematical register. In Better Buy, as in Weigh
up, relationship was the focus of the question. Students responded
by using verbs that emphasised this relationship, usually in the
form of 'it costs more', 'it's heavier', 'it's lighter', or 'it's
second lightest'.
For both age
groups the second most common classification of verbs were static
ones, such as 'is'. These were used in statements such as 'this
is better'. Similarly, for both 24 age groups mathematical verbs
were the third most common category. There was a higher proportion
of mathematical verbs for Year 8 students than Year 4 students.
For Better Buy, the mathematical terms included 'costs', plus',
'equals', and 'times'. In the early years of school, students are
encouraged to use everyday language for mathematical terms, but
by Year 4 this is expected to have been phased out with mathematical
terms being used instead (Learning Media, 1992). However, these
transcripts showed that students continued to use both everyday
and mathematical language, and for this task, everyday language,
including pronouns for agents and static verbs were more prevalent
than mathematical ones. |
|
|
|
Text
Structures |
|
|
Our
results from counting agents and processes gave us confusing results,
so we reexamined the responses and classified them according to the
accuracy of the response and the clarity of the language. As can be
seen in Table 3.5, the explanations that students gave in describing
their reasoning for which box was the better buy do not always show
that clear language is used to explain the correct answer. Nor was
it clear that if you used clear language that you were likely to have
the correct answer. This supports the suggestion by Ellerton and Clarkson
(1996) that there is no simple relationship between good mathematics
understanding and good mathematical language. However, as Table 3.5
shows, it was much more likely that students in Year 8 who answered
the question correctly were able to clearly explain their reasoning.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
It
is clear that students who are most likely to give clear descriptions
of their accurate reasoning were those students from high decile
schools, although a number of students from middle decile schools
were also able to do this. Students from low decile schools were
less likely to have reasoned the answer correctly and to have clear
25 language. Slightly more boys gave an accurate response than girls,
whilst in the lowest level of accuracy, slightly more girls did
not compare size and price than boys.
Although Table
3.5 provides some information about the differences between groups
in the language they used to explain their answers, it was not sufficient
to understand what were the essential parts of a clear, accurate
response. It was decided therefore to investigate the features of
clear language so that the typical structure of children's explanations
could be identified. Using the ideas of Hasan (Halliday and Hasan,
1985), we looked for text elements and how they were combined to
form a justification of their calculation.
From interrogating
the data, it was clear that every student's explanation contained
one or more or the following three features. These were Premise,
Consequence and Conclusion. Premises are statements
of ideas upon which the student's reasoning is built. These are
the linguistic equivalences of Krummheuer's (1995) grounds which
were described in Chapter 1. The students in responding to this
task used two types of Premises. One was the repetition of a fact
that was given in the question, such as 'It's fifty grams and that's
100 grams'. These were labelled as Factual Premises and were also
seen in Motorway task. The other Premise was when a hypothetical
situation was mooted, such as 'If I buy two of them …'. Descriptions
of the ideas built on these Premises are labelled as consequences.
For example:
Because
if you buy two of these boxes, it's going to equal a
hundred grams and only cost a dollar twenty. |
Premise |
Consequence |
implicit
Conclusion |
|
The final feature
of these explanations was a Conclusion. This is where the student
made a reference to better value. Only nine students used an explicit
Conclusion in their response. However, 25 other students used words
such as 'more', 'only', 'but' to cue the listener to the fact that
a comparison had been made. These were labelled as implicit Conclusions.
Given that these were oral explanations where the context was shared
between the child and the teacher administrator, it is to be expected
that the listener would have to supply some background information
to what they were being told (Halliday, 1985). It is perhaps more
surprising that some students chose to be so explicit in their reasoning.
If the Conclusion came before the Premise (and the Consequence),
then the student was most likely pre-empting the question asking
for their reasoning when they responded to the question about which
box was better value.
The students
used one of ten different combinations in giving their reasoning.
Table 3.6 provides examples of each of these combinations and the
number of students who used the different types of Premises. In
the examples, Q stands for a question or prompt from the teacher
administrator. |
|
|
|
|
|
|