Table
4.42 shows that, on the whole, combinations with elaborators were
more likely to be used by girls than by boys. This was particularly
so in the combinations of an Elaborator with a Premise or a Physical
Consequence. However, Elaborators with Conclusions were most likely
to be used by boys, although the numbers of students who used this
combination were quite small. The distribution of Elaborators used
with Consequences and Suppositions was dependent on the section
of the task. When boys did use proportionally more Elaborators with
other elements than girls, the differences were not as great as
when girls used more Elaborators than boys.
The use of Physical
Consequences is, unsurprisingly, closely related to when students
were actually performing actions. This shows up in the large numbers
of students who used Physical Consequences in their response to
the Plan and to the Description parts of this task. There is also
a relationship between the combination of Physical Consequence and
Conclusion and clarity of language in the responses to the Description.
94% of students who were deemed to have given an accurate and clear
response to this task included this combination in their response.
As was discussed
in Chapter 1, Bills (2002) suggested that particular words in students'
descriptions of mental computations correlated with students providing
accurate answers. In considering the results from the three parts
of this task, it can be said that certain combinations of text elements
are also related to students providing accurate responses. The Premise
- Consequence - Elaborator was used by a significant number of students
in responding to each part of the task. In the responses to the
Plan, 40% of students used this combination. In the Description
part of the task, 26% of students used it and in the Explanation
part of the task 53% of students used it. Students who were considered
to be accurate were more likely to use this combination than students
who were only approaching accuracy. Although Tables 4.13, 4.26 and
4.40 suggest that Premise - Consequence combinations were important,
they also show that it was more likely that these combinations were
followed by an Elaborator than not.
As well as the
Premise - Consequence - Elaborator combination being important,
in the Description part of the task, students who gave accurate
and clear responses almost all used a Physical Consequence - Consequence
combination. In mathematical explanations, when physical actions
are needed, it would benefit students if they learnt to use this
combination both to support the clarity of their language but also
to help their thinking. This is because they move from just describing
the consequences of their actions to drawing a logical consequence
from the results of that action.
There also seemed
to be some distinct trends in the use of logical connectives across
the different parts of this task, which are related to how different
text combinations were used. Certainly when the Physical Consequence
- Consequence combination was used in the Description responses,
more often than not this was joined with 'so' suggesting a causal
relationship between the ideas expressed in the Physical Consequence
and the Consequence.
Causality is
not shown through logical connectives joining Premises to Consequences
as had been the case in some of the other tasks. Girls and Year
8 students were the most likely to use a logical connective in this
position, with 'and' being the most common logical connective. Instead,
causality was marked through the use of 'if' in front of a Premise
or an Elaborator in the Premise - Consequence - Elaborator combination.
If 'if' was not used, then the causality had to be implied from
the relative clause which was the Elaborator. For example, in 'take
two and measure them first, and see which one's the heaviest, and
then put the heaviest one up on the heaviest end', 'which one's
the heaviest' is the relative clause which was coded as an Elaborator.
It presumes that the outcome of finding which box is the heavier
is easily done and so it can be incorporated into the following
suggestion which was 'and then put the heaviest one up on the heaviest
end'. Thus the causality is between the proposed action and what
follows. Contrast this with 'then weigh them all, all of them to
see if that's the lightest, and then just do it to all of them'.
In this case the 'if' at the beginning of the Elaborator marks this
causal relationship in a more pronounced way. However, more has
to be presumed from the original action for the final result to
be valid. Both sorts of Elaborators allow students to deal with
possibilities in a succinct way so that further actions can then
be described. It would seem that students used the Premise - Consequence
- Elaborator combination as one way of dealing with possibilities.
In order to
discuss in a clear and concise manner the possible ordering of the
four boxes, students needed ways of talking about possibilities.
This has many links to the ability to use generalisations, which
are important aspects of mathematical explanations and justifications.
In order to interpret and use them correctly, students need to know
the constraints on an equation being true.
In this task,
both the Plan and the Explanation sections required students to
describe general ways of ordering the boxes. Anthony and Walshaw
(2002) suggested that there was a need 'to discern generality in
students' informal utterances' and to understand '[t]he interplay
between generalisation and justification' (p. 52). By analysing
their responses, it is possible to understand how typical primary
students limited their generalisations so that they remained true.
The next few paragraphs describe first the Premise - Consequence
- Elaborator combination and how the logical connectives and other
aspects of this combination limit when the possibilities were true.
In many cases, this combination of text elements could be considered
as examples of Bills and Grey's (2001) 'general' responses, as they
provided a rule which would cover most instances. Then Suppositions,
especially students' use of 'say' could be related to Bills and
Grey's 'generic' responses, as they were used to provide examples
of how these rules operated. Students who were only able to talk
about the specific boxes in front of them would be considered as
giving 'particular' responses. From Tables 4.13, 4.26, 4.40, there
would seem to be a relationship between the use of Premise - Consequence
- Elaborator combinations and/or the use of Suppositions with the
responses which were deemed accurate and clear.
Tables 4.13,
4.26 and 4.40 show that more students used the Premise - Consequence
- Elaborator combination when giving their Explanations, with the
least number of students doing so when giving their responses to
the Description part of the task. This supports the belief that
students used this combination so that they could talk about the
possibilities in ordering the boxes. The Premise - Consequence -
Elaborator allows an action to be proposed and the consequent result
of that action to be carried forward into the step, even though
the result may not be definite. As was described earlier, sometimes
a logical connective such as 'if' is used to mark the conditions
under which the following action would be true. However, a relative
clause, such as 'which is heavier', was also used where the conditions
for the statement to be considered true are within the object itself.
Students also
used Suppositions, such as 'say', to mark a possibility. Although
the number of students using Suppositions was similar, the expressions
used did differ across each section of Weigh Up. In responses to
the Plan part, four students used 'say'. In their responses to the
Description part only one student used 'say', whilst in their Explanation
responses, five students used 'say'. By using 'say', students marked
the fact that it was a possibility rather than a fact, as in the
following: 'so say it was D, you put it in front of it'. 'Suppose'
and 'imagine' were more Standard English words which also indicate
that what was following was a proposition rather than a fact. However,
only two students throughout the task used these expressions. It
is interesting to note that these expressions which mark possibilities
are verbs.
As well as verbs,
adverbs such as 'perhaps' and 'probably' were also used occasionally
to indicate a possibility. Although four students used these expressions
in their responses to the Plan and another two students used them
in their responses to the Description, no student used them in their
responses to the Explanation. This would suggest that these adverbs
have only a limited role in describing possibilities. However, when
the actual examples such as the one from a Year 8 girl from a high
decile school are examined this is not the case. The example was
'say B's the heaviest so A's lighter, I'd do that later, I'd perhaps
put D, perhaps B's still heavier, so throw out D, then C, perhaps
C's heavier'. Here 'perhaps' could be replaced by 'say' and the
meaning would remain the same.
The large numbers
of students who used 'you think' in the responses to the Explanation
seemed to have used them like 'say'. This can be seen in the following
example from a Year 8 boy from a high decile school: 'See how heavy
it is in your hands first and then you put the two that you think
are the lightest in'. Once again an action is suggested and the
consequent result, although not a certainty, is carried forward
into the following action. Although 'think' as a verb is also used
to mark a possibility rather than an actual fact, the inclusion
of the pronoun suggests that a person, 'you' could actually change
the certainty of the actual result. 'Say' puts forward a possibility
so that a proposed action can be described which could be used with
any set of four boxes. 'You think' on the other hand is used to
be more definite about those particular boxes.
However, the
use of 'I think' in the responses although also coded as Suppositions
does not appear to mark possibilities. Instead, it is used to highlight
uncertainty about actual results of actions such as in the following
from a Year 8 boy attending a high decile school: 'I think that
that one is heavier than that one'.
Being able to
describe possibilities is one way of being able to define the conditions
under which certain statements are true. This is an important component
of being able to describe mathematical generalisations. Although
Esty (1992) suggested that this is mainly done through the use of
logical connectives, this would seem to be only one way that primary
school children do this. Although they can provide information on
the conditions under which a statement is true through the use of
'if' at the beginning of an Elaborator in a Premise - Consequence
- Elaborator combination, possibilities can also be described through
the use of relative clauses as Elaborators in these combinations
or by the use of 'say', 'perhaps' or 'probably' as markers of possibilities.
The Weigh Up
task was a long, extended task with three related parts. Students'
responses to these different parts showed that the structure of
mathematical explanations do differ as the task requirements differ.
Although the Premise was a text element found in all other tasks,
some students, in responding to the Description, did not include
a Premise. Instead a new element, Physical Consequence, was more
commonly used by students in responding to this part of the task.
It would also seem that students who were considered to give accurate
and clear responses to this part of the task were most likely to
combine Physical Consequences with Consequences. In the other two
sections of the task, a Premise - Consequence - Elaborator combination
was used by many students who gave clear, accurate responses. This
has implications for the types of structures that may support students'
being able to provide clear accurate responses, but there is also
a need to make students aware of the type of tasks which are most
likely to require different text combinations. |