aTask
and acceptable answers |
Form
of the questions
For this task, the instructions were: |
|
Show
student photo (a picture of cars on a motorway)
|
This
picture shows a busy motorway. During the day time, about
98 cars go down this road in every minute. |
1.
About how many cars would go down the road in 9 minutes? |
Record
student's answer |
2.
Explain to me how you got your answer. |
|
|
Figure
5.1: Question for Motorway task |
|
|
The form of the questions suggests that a number is expected for the
answer to the first question and a description of mathematical manipulation
for the second, such as '98 is nearly 100, and I multiplied 9 times
100 to get 900, so it would be a bit less than 900'. An answer that
ignored the personal aspect of the explanation would also be acceptable.
Such an answer might say 'Nine 100s is 900'. |
|
|
Answers
that were approximately correct |
For
this analysis, we considered answers between 800 and 1015 to be
reasonably accurate, as was a clear statement that the answer was
98 times 9. This level of accuracy is different from that credited
by NEMP, which limited the range of credited answers to those between
850 and 900. We chose a somewhat more lax range in analysing language,
as it gave us more students whose answers were likely to include
mathematical language. The major difference in success on this item,
by our criteria, was between Year 4 and Year 8 students.
Of the Year
4 students, 9 gave answers that were within this range. Correct
answers came from each group analysed. Three of the six high decile
girls gave approximately correct answers, while in other groups
one or two students were relatively accurate. Half of the appropriate
answers came from the high decile students.
A total of 29
Year 8 students gave answers in this range or gave the calculation
as 98 times 9. Again, some correct answers were given by students
in each subgroup, between 3 correct answers (low decile non-Pacific
boys) and 6 correct answers (high decile girls). There was no overall
pattern for success by gender, ethnicity or economic background
in the proportion of students who were reasonably correct. See Table
5.1. |
|
|
|
Table
5.1. Number of students in each subgroup who were approximately
accurate on the Motorway task |
|
|
|
Pacific
Low decile |
Non-Pacific
Low decile |
High
decile |
 |
Year
4 |
Boys |
1 |
2 |
2 |
Year 4
|
Girls |
1 |
1 |
3 |
Year 8
|
Boys |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Year 8
|
Girls |
5 |
4 |
6 |
 |
|
|
|
Form
of the answers |
The
answers that students gave could be classified as numerical, mathematical
explanation, non-mathematical stories, and statements that indicated
not knowing or uncertainty (e.g. 'I guessed', 'don't know').
All but one
student gave a numerical answer. For Year 4 students, these answers
ranged between 90 and 1 000 000. For the Year 8 students, the answers
ranged between 108 and 48000. Two Year 8 student said that the answer
would be ninetyeight times nine and did not estimate what 98 x 9
would equal. |
|
|
Explanation
types of Year 4 and Year 8 Students |
There
was a difference between groups in the nature of responses given to
the request for an explanation, as shown in the following table. |
|
|
|
Table
5.2. Number of students giving a mathematical explanation (whether
or not it was correct) |
|
|
|
Mathematical
explanation |
Non-mathematical
story |
No
explanation, including guessed, don't know |
 |
Year
4 |
Pacific
Low Decile |
1 |
3 |
8 |
|
Non-Pacific
Low Decile |
7 |
3 |
2 |
|
High Decile |
8 |
1 |
3 |
Year 8
|
Pacific
Low Decile |
9 |
1 |
2 |
|
Non-Pacific
Low Decile |
10 |
1 |
1 |
|
High Decile |
12 |
0 |
0 |
 |
|
|
|
Year
8 students gave markedly more mathematical explanations, a finding
that is consistent with their greater accuracy. Only one Pacific,
low decile, Year 4 student attempted to give a mathematical explanation,
while the majority of all other groups at least alluded to mathematical
operations. The majority of Year 4 Pacific students did not attempt
an answer, saying 'I guessed' or saying 'no' when asked for an explanation.
|
|
|
Non-mathematical
stories |
Both
low and high decile year 4 students gave stories without mathematical
content. The nature of these stories is covered more fully in Chapter
7. The stories of Year 4 students included:
I got no idea.
Oh, because they busy? And they waiting for the stop at traffic
light. [Q] Because there's many cars, there's heaps. [Q] And they're
waiting at the traffic not working, won't work, and that's all
(Year 4 Pacific low decile boy)
Because the
motorway's kind of busy and people don't really umm hold up on
the motorway, because you can kind of, because there basically
isn't a speed limit on the motorway, they go quite fast. (High
decile Year 4 girl)
The two Year
8 non-mathematical stories could be considered protoquantitative,
using the terms 'too many' and 'lots of' to describe the reason
why there would be many cars.
because there's
too many cars. [Q] there's too many cars in the traffic (Year
8 low decile non-Pacific girl)
None of stories
told by the Year 4 students had a numerical answer that was approximately
right. For the three stories given above, the numerical answers
were 89, 52 and a 1000 respectively. The Year 8 student had an approximation
of 1000, which could indicate some number sense in the absence of
the ability to explain.
In addition
to these explanatory stories, some Year 4 students and some interviewers
personalised the story. These examples were conversational. For
example, one interviewer said 'That's a lot more cars than go by
here, isn't it'. A Pacific Year 4 girl asked, 'Is this New Zealand?'
Another conversational example came from a high decile boy who said,
'if it was Auckland, it would be a typical day', and then moved
to working with decontextualised numbers. This discussion about
the picture did not occur with Year 8 students, and may have been
an attempt to keep rapport going with the younger students.
For the Year
4 students, these stories suggest that, in the absence of a ready
mathematical explanation, a story about the context seemed the best
answer to these students. As with the stories told by different
economic groups in response to the Bank Account task, the stories
were related to their personal experience. |
|
|
Mathematical
explanations |
How
one does a mathematical operation mentally is not easy to explain,
as found in experience with the Numeracy Project (2004). Explanations
considered mathematical among those given by these students ranged
between protoquantitative ones that did not include numbers and
said 'many more' or 'heaps…more', and those that mentioned an operation
like 'count them' to those that explained the multiplication followed
by repeated subtraction of 2. Examples of these are given in Table
5.3.
Of the nine
reasonably correct answers from Year 4 students, four were accompanied
by explanations that could be followed. It is possible that the
other reasonable answers were guesses that could not be explained
or that the student found them in an appropriate manner but then
could not explain what they had done. All of the Year 8 accurate
answers were easy to follow and usually concise. |
|
|
|
Table
5.3. Examples of categories of mathematical explanations given
by Year 4 students. [answer in square brackets] |
|
|
Protoquantitative
or vague |
Count,
add |
inaccurate
algorithm |
Table |
Add
and mult |
Add,
Mult + subtr |
 |
Heaps
more[200] Yr4 PG |
Started
repeated addition, can't do it [998]- Yr4HG |
9x9=81,8x8=7
2, put together=[153] Yr4LB |
90x10 [900]
-Yr4HB |
90+90=100smt
g, [1000] Yr4HB |
doubles/then
980, 992,then repeated subtraction of 2 [882] Yr4HG |
8 is back
from 9 so you go forward [99]- Yr4 HB |
Count them
[120] Yr4LG |
Hand version
of 9 times table [143]- Yr4HB |
Times 98
x 9 done in head without talk [882] Yr8LnPG |
9x9+1x10+1+9
-[909] Yr4HG |
98+2=100,
9 100s is 900-2 9 times [888] Yr4 LB |
About 1000
- no explanation Yr4 |
take away
some [40] Yr4LG |
9 x 98
and then plussed the 2 (sub)answers together [140]- Yr8HB |
|
190+188
until you get 90smtg- [990] Yr4LG, |
900, counted
all the 2s and minused that -[882]Yr8HB |
98 in 1
min, 1/2 would be still coming [1015] Yr4LB |
counted
on fingers and missed out 2 ones [600] Yr4LG |
|
|
98+98,
take 100 and mult by 8 more [1000]-Yr8 HG |
|
That's
probably roughly what can go down [48000] Yr8LnPB |
thought
100, 300, put 98 on that [398] Yr4 LG |
|
|
|
|
|
Plussed
98 with 8 more 98s [872] Yr48PB Because you count it [108] Yr8LPB
|
|
|
|
|
 |
|
|
|
The
relatively accurate answers mostly describe a combination of multiplication,
addition and subtraction. Given the fact that these students were
describing a mental function that was sometimes lengthy, some of
these explanations were remarkably good. Even conceptually clear
answers included restarted phrases (I did nine, ninety times nine)
and a second start at the sentence as below.
Well, I did
nine, ninety nine times nine, I thought well I'm not going into
ninety times nine so I just did ninety times ten is nine hundred
so. (Year 4 high decile boy)
Note that in
this section the boy uses 'did' as the verb to describe his mathematical
manipulation rather than 'multiplied', and a colloquial 'I'm not
going in to' to describe a choice abandoned.
Another well-described
procedure was:
I just um,
went up to 98 plus two is a hundred and nine a hundreds are nine
hundred take away.. two nines, oh, um… [Q] Um, I forgot how I
done that .. um, it was... [Q] Ninety eight, plus two is a hundred
and nine a hundreds are nine hundred take away two, and then,
um, take away two nines and I got eight hundred and eighty eight.
(Year 4 low decile non-Pacific boy)
The only absolutely
accurate answer from a Year 4 student started by describing one
method for multiplication, repeated doubling, and then moved to
repeated subtraction. This boy thought out loud showing how he found
his answer before giving being asked for an explanation. His explanation
includes self-corrections and shaking his head when he decided that
his way of starting was not going to be useful. ...
(silent, thinking,
then counts very quietly to himself)...which makes a hundred and
eighty, six, another one that makes a hundred and eighty .. I
mean a hundred and ninety, six, and a hundred and ninety four
.. wait (shaking his head)...nine hundred, nine hundred and eighty,
nine hundred and ninety two .. eight hundred and ninety .. ninety
eight, eight hundred and ninety six, eight hundred and ninety
four, eight hundred and ninety two, eight hundred and ninety,
eight hundred and eighty eight, eight hundred and eighty six,
eight hundred and eighty four, eight hundred and eighty two, eight
hundred and eighty two.
Q
Um well, what I did is I added up, um, I went from nine, nine
hundred from ninety eight and I made that a hundred then I subtracted
nine, nine twos from it, from the answer.
In this protocol
'makes' is used for 'equals'.
Other students
who gave accurate answers gave considerably less clear explanations,
possibly because they were not able to recall how they did the problem.
For example, a girl who gave her answer as 998 started using repeated
addition in her explanation and then gave up. We are left uncertain
about how she got her answer, which could have been done by multiplying
9 x 10 and adding 98.
Umm .. ninety
eight, plus ninety eight, plus ninety eight, plus ninety eight,
plus ninety eight, plus ninety eight, plus ninety eight, no, I
can't do it. (Year 4 high decile girl)
Another student
who gave 1000 as his answer said:
Because um,
ninety and ninety makes a hundred and something .. an then there'll
just be ten hundred.' (Year 4 high decile boy)
Incorrect answers
sometimes included a recognisable procedure. One student described
the way in which nine times tables could be worked out on your fingers
or knuckles. Another student described the vertical algorithm, but
added the products ignoring the fact that the first amount was not
9 times 9 but 90 times 9
. …it's fifty,
a hundred and um fifty three.
Q
..um, because um, nine times nine is eighty one, and the ninth
um eight times nine is seventy two, and I put it together. (Year
4 low decile non-Pacific boy)
The most common
answers from Year 8 students were versions of ninety-eight times
nine or 100 times 9. These students did not appear to have as much
difficulty in explaining the mathematical path to their solution
as the Year 4 students did. |
|
|
Linguistic
elements of Year 4 and Year 8 students' answers |
This
question aroused less discussion than did the other items. As a
quick index of the amount of talk from each ethnic and economic
group, the number of linguistic elements recorded was totalled.
For high decile students, 270 elements were recorded; for low decile
non-Pacific students, 184 elements were recorded; and for low decile
Pacific students, 100 elements were recorded. Although rough, this
is one index of the willingness of these students to discuss mathematics
with the interviewers. It is compatible with the argument that found
that students are more likely to carry on conversations with people
from similar backgrounds than with people of different backgrounds
(Bogdan & Biklen, 1982).
In the following
section, totals usually equal more than 100% as students used more
than one type of each element in their answers. |
|
|
Agents |
The
form of the second question tells the student to: 'Explain to me
how you got your answer'. Thus it would be conversationally
appropriate for a student to use a personal pronoun in their answer.
Nancy Barron
(1971) found that men and women teachers chose to use different
grammatical cases in their classroom discussions. She hypothesised
that this reflected differences in what was valued by the different
gender groups.
Women produced
a significantly greater proportion of explicit participative cases
than men, thus demonstrating their greater concern with internal
physiological states. The greater involvement of men with implementation
of action by means of objects was shown by their greater use of
instrumental or source cases. (p. 39)
As a result
of this, it seemed interesting to code who or what was the main
actor in the majority of clauses of each response. For this task,
the actors were either 'I', a generic 'you' or an impersonal actor
such as a number or calculation or the question itself. These final
type of impersonal actors are discussed further in the section on
hesitant language. The distribution of these is given in the following
table. |
|
|
|
Table
5.4. Main actor in responses. |
|
|
Actor |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
I |
15 |
14 |
12 |
17 |
8 |
9 |
12 |
29 |
you |
5 |
4 |
5 |
4 |
3 |
2 |
4 |
9 |
impersonal |
18 |
18 |
22 |
14 |
16 |
11 |
9 |
36 |
 |
|
|
|
Certainly
the gender difference that Barron had found with teachers was not
found with students when responding to this task.
Sixty-one percent
of the Year 4 students and 81% of the Year 8 students used 'I',
'you', or an implied personal pronoun as agents in their explanations.
Among Year 4 students, Pacific students were more likely to use
either 'it' or an existential agent (e.g. 'there are'). This difference
is significant (Fisher's exact test 2-tailed p=0.0031). For Year
8 students there was almost no difference amongst groups.
Twenty-two,
or 60% of Year 8 students used numbers as the agent of their sentences
or clauses, as in 'because 98 plus 2…', while only six (17% of the
Year 4 students) limited their explanations to having the numbers
as the agent. For both year levels, this use was spread across the
groups. The difference appeared to be related to competence in the
Year 8 students' ability to strip away the story shell and deal
with the mathematics only, while the younger students talked about
what they did or what the cars did. |
|
|
Processes |
This
was the one task of the four considered in this study for which
mathematical verbs predominated for both year levels. These verbs
included 'times', 'take away', 'equals' and 'estimate'. One third
(33%) of all verbs by Year 4 students and nearly half (48%) of those
used by Year 8 students used were considered mathematical. The second
largest category was static verbs.
In Year 4,
this preponderance of mathematical verbs over all other types of
verbs is mostly due to the upper decile students who said more and
gave more mathematical explanations (Fisher's 2-tailed test p =
0.0472).
In Year 8,
the highest proportion of mathematical verbs was used by low decile
non- Pacific boys but this proportion was influenced by a high number
used by two of the boys who gave extensive answers. See Table 5.5.
|
|
|
|
Table
5.5. Types of action terms (verbs) used by each main category
(combining girls and boys). |
|
|
|
Total
action terms (verbs) |
Mathematical |
Static |
Non-mathematical
|
Other |
 |
Yr
4 High decile |
79 |
37 |
29 |
7 |
6 |
Yr 4 Low non-Pacific |
57 |
17 |
13 |
15 |
12 |
Yr 4 Low Pacific |
30 |
0 |
12 |
14 |
4 |
Yr 8 High
decile |
70 |
32 |
61 |
4 |
7 |
Yr 8 Low non-Pacific |
73 |
31 |
28 |
7 |
8 |
Yr 8 Low Pacific |
25 |
18 |
6 |
0 |
1 |
 |
|
|
|
Abstractions
and names of numbers |
As
might be expected in a problem where numbers were given, the names
of numbers were used much more frequently than abstractions like 'all'
or 'every'. This was true for both economic groups, both ethnic groups
and both genders in Year 4, but was only used by one student amongst
all those in Year 8. |
|
|
Comparison |
Expressions
of comparison were made only by those Year 4 students who did not
work out the mathematics of the problem, and by none of the Year 8
students. An example was '… if there's so many go down in one minute
will there be so many more have to go down in nine minutes' (Year
4 high decile girl). |
|
|
Connectives |
In
response to this question, Year 8 students used more causal connectives
('because', 'so') than additive ones (primarily 'and'). The overall
numbers were small, with 31 causal connectives and 27 additive connectives
being used by the Year 4 students. The Year 8 students used a total
of 43 causal connectives, most of which were 'because' and 'so', and
51 additive connectives, 36 of which were 'and'. Where 'and' was used
repeatedly, this was usually because the multiplication problem was
done as repeated addition. Causal connectives are appropriate for
mathematical explanations. An example of this use was, 'you'd use
a times so you would do ninety times, ninety times, ninety times nine…´(Year
4 high decile boy). |
|
|
aText
structure |
Motorway,
like Weigh Up, requires students to explain what they did, in this
case to get the number of cars which go down the Motorway. Table 5.6
shows which students used clear language in giving their explanation.
LPG stands for Pacific girl attending a low decile school. LnPB stands
for a non-Pacific student who attends a low decile school, whereas
HG stands for girls attending high decile schools. The spread is much
more complicated that that for Better Buy. This is not just because
the students were using a range of different strategies for solving
the problem, but there was also far greater spread of answers which
were categorised as clear, moderately clear, vague or elliptical.
Once again, it was not always those students who had an accurate answer
or close estimate who had the clear language. Bills (2002) described
work by Piaget (1928), who felt that having been asked 'how they had
performed a calculation, after giving an answer by some automatic
process, children may invent something which would give rise to the
same answer' (p. 99). It may well be that, as some students' explanations
did not always seem to match the amounts they gave, that similar inventions
of explanation also occurred in this task. |
|
|
|
Table
5.6. Clarity of language vs. accuracy for explanation*.
|
|
 |
|
Clear
Language |
Moderately
clear but some specific information missing |
Unclear,
vague |
Ellipitical |
 |
Uses
rounding to provide an accurate answer |
1Yr
8 LnPB |
1
Yr 8 LPG |
|
|
1
Yr 4 LnPB |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Uses
rounding to achieve an estimate |
1
Yr 8 LPG |
3
Yr 8 HG |
1
Yr 8 LPB |
1
Yr 8 LnPG |
|
1
Yr 8 LnPG |
1
Yr 8 HB |
1
Yr 8 HB |
|
|
2
Yr 8 HG |
1
Yr 4 LnPG |
|
|
|
1
Yr 8 HB |
|
|
|
|
1Yr
4 HB |
|
|
|
|
1
Yr 4 HG |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
9 x 98 |
2
Yr 8LPG |
1
Yr 8 LnPB |
|
1
Yr 8 HG |
|
2
Yr 8 PLB |
1
Yr 8 LnPG |
|
|
|
3
Yr 8 LnPG |
1
Yr 4 HG |
|
|
|
2
Yr 8 HB |
|
|
|
|
1Yr
4 LnPB |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Other mathematical
calculations |
1
Yr 8 PLB |
2
Yr 8 LnPBs |
1
Yr 8 LnPB |
1
Yr 4 HB |
|
1
Yr 8 LnPG |
1
Yr 4 HG |
1
Yr 8 HB |
1
Yr 4 LnPG |
|
|
|
1
Yr 4 LnPB |
|
|
|
|
1
Yr 4 LnPG |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Doesn't
use information mathematically |
1
Yr 8LPG |
1
Yr 4 LnPG |
6
Yr 4 LPGs |
1
Yr 8 LPB |
|
1
Yr 8 LPB |
1
Yr 4 HB |
4
Yr 4 LPB |
1
Yr 4 LPG |
|
1
Yr 8 LnPB |
|
1
Yr 4 LnPG |
1
Yr 4 LPB |
|
1 Yr 4
LnPB |
|
2
Yr 4 LnPBs |
1
Yr 4 LnPG |
|
1 Yr 4
HB |
|
3
Yr 4 HG |
1
Yr 4 HB |
|
|
|
1
Yr 4 HB |
|
 |
*Student
identification as in Table 3.1 |
|
|
|
There
were 18 responses which used estimations to gain either an approximate
or exact amount. Over half of these were by students attending high
decile schools, twice as many students as those from low decile
schools, either Pacific Islanders or not. There were equivalent
numbers of boys and girls, but more than three quarters were students
in Year 8. Similar results for year level and gender can be seen
for the 14 responses which gave '9 x 98' for their calculation.
However, there were no significant differences in the decile level
of schools attended. At the other end of the table, of the 20 students
who were unable to use information mathematically, 7 students were
from high decile schools. Six students were from low decile schools
in Pacific communities. There was a similar even distribution by
gender. One difference in distribution was that 16 of these 20 students
were in Year 4. This suggests that students from high decile schools
were most likely to use estimates whilst non-mathematical responses
were more evenly spread across groups.
Of 26 students
who used clear language, 20 were in Year 8, suggesting that clear
language is related to age regardless of the mathematical ability
of the student. Students were clearer in Year 8 whether they gave
an appropriate mathematical response or not. There were no other
differences according to gender, decile level of school attended
or ethnicity. There were only 8 students who gave elliptical responses
with no clear distinctions between any group.
The Motorway
task required students to provide an explanation of their calculation
to determine the number of cars. As such, it shared many similarities
with the Weigh Up task which also required an explanation in its
Plan and Explanation sections. It was, therefore, not surprising
to find many of the same text elements being used; that is Premise,
Consequences, Conclusions and Elaborators. The distribution of students
using these elements is given in Table 5.7. |
|
|
|
Table
5.7. Use of text elements by different groups. |
|
|
Text
Elements |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
introduction |
5 |
6 |
4 |
7 |
1 |
6 |
4 |
11 |
premise |
36 |
36 |
36 |
36 |
24 |
24 |
24 |
72 |
consequence |
15 |
14 |
13 |
16 |
3 |
10 |
16 |
29 |
conclusion |
7 |
10 |
8 |
3 |
0 |
7 |
10 |
17 |
 |
|
|
|
Apart
from Introductions which appear to be used more by Year 8 students
(and the numbers are not large), there do not appear to be differences
in distribution of students based on gender or age for any of the
text elements. However, there are significant differences due to ethnicity.
On the whole, Pacific students did not use any text elements apart
from Premises. Non-Pacific students who attended a similar decile
level of school used slightly less Consequences and Conclusions than
students attending high decile schools. Interestingly, it would seem
that these students used more Introductions than those from high decile
schools. However, the numbers are small and no clear trends can be
determined. These differences between Pacific students and others
can be seen clearly in Table 5.8, which shows which students used
particular text combinations. |
|
|
|
Table
5.8. Use of combinations of text elements by different groups. |
|
|
Text
Structures |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
intro
- premise |
3 |
0 |
0 |
3 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
3 |
intro
- premise – consequence (+ conclusion) |
2 |
6 |
4 |
4 |
0 |
4 |
4 |
8 |
premise |
17 |
18 |
20 |
15 |
20 |
10 |
5 |
35 |
premise
– consequence |
8 |
7 |
7 |
8 |
3 |
4 |
8 |
15 |
premise
– consequence – conclusion |
5 |
1 |
2 |
4 |
0 |
2 |
4 |
6 |
premise
– conclusion |
1 |
4 |
3 |
2 |
0 |
2 |
3 |
5 |
|
|
|
More
students attending high decile schools used combinations of three
or more text elements. As was seen in Table 5.6, Pacific students
predominantly only used a Premise. The next few tables go through
each of the text elements and provide more details before looking
at what combinations of text elements were most likely to be related
to clear language and accurate responses.
Introductions
only occurred at the beginning of the response and seemed to personalise
it. For example, one boy stated
Um, I go for,
I go to nine times nine then eight times nine, and then, then
I, then I plussed the two answers together and got around, that.
'I go to' is
setting the scene in that he is describing what he did before going
on to describe the actual process.
As seen in Table
5.9, eleven students began their responses with an Introduction.
All were followed by a Premise and two continued with a Consequence,
while another six used a Premise, a Consequence and a Conclusion.
It was, therefore, more likely that an Introduction was found in
texts which incorporated a longer, more complex series of elements.
|
|
|
|
Table
5.9. Use of Introductions by different groups. |
|
|
Text
Structures |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
introduction
– premise – consequence – conclusion |
1 |
5 |
3 |
3 |
0 |
3 |
3 |
6 |
introduction
– premise – consequence |
1 |
1 |
1 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
1 |
2 |
introduction
– premise |
3 |
0 |
0 |
3 |
1 |
2 |
0 |
3 |
|
|
|
These
numbers are small with no clear trends based on gender or decile level
of school attended. It would seem that Year 8 students were more likely
to employ an Introduction than Year 4 and students from high decile
schools were more likely to use them than students from Pacific communities,
in low-decile schools. The distribution of groups using an Introduction
are similar to those without an Introduction as can be seen in Table
5.8. |
|
|
|
Table
5.10. Use of Conclusions by different groups
(including those with Introductions). |
|
|
Conclusions |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
Conclusion
preceded by consequence |
6 |
6 |
5 |
7 |
0 |
5 |
7 |
12 |
Conclusion
preceded by premise |
1 |
4 |
3 |
2 |
0 |
2 |
3 |
5 |
|
|
|
In
Table 5.10 there are no clear distinctions in who used Conclusions
on the basis of age, although there does seem to be a slight tendency
for boys rather than girls to use them. No Conclusions were found
at the beginning of text structures, unlike what had occurred in
the Better Buy responses. It was far more likely that students from
high decile schools included them in their responses. Unlike the
Better Buy texts, only two of these Conclusions were implicit. One
other small difference is that Year 8 students were more likely
to have a Conclusion preceded by a Consequence, whereas a Conclusion
preceded only by a Premise was more likely to be used by a Year
4 student. However, the numbers are small and any pattern remains
uncertain.
Table 5.11 shows
the distribution of students who concluded their responses with
a Consequence. As there were differences between Premises, the type
of preceding Premise is also provided. Premises are divided into
types based on the kind of evidence they used. A factual Premise
used information given in the question or from the photo which was
supplied, for example 'because it says nine minutes', 'because there's
lots of cars going up and down'. A mathematical Premise used a mathematical
calculation or made reference to a mathematical fact, such as 'I
rounded ninety-eight up to, umm, one hundred' or 'ninety and ninety
makes a hundred and something'. When students used more than one
Premise, they were categorised under the first Premise or the type
of Premise used most often. |
|
|
|
Table
5.11. Use of Consequences as the final element and their preceding
Premise (includes responses with an Introduction). |
|
|
Consequences |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
consequence
preceded by factual premise |
3 |
1 |
2 |
2 |
2 |
0 |
2 |
4 |
consequence
preceded by mathematical premise |
6 |
7 |
6 |
7 |
1 |
5 |
7 |
13 |
|
|
|
Once
again, the numbers are small with few distinguishing differences between
groups. Similar numbers of Year 4 and Year 8 students used this structure.
However, there were more students using a mathematical Premise than
a factual Premise and these extra students were more likely to be
boys, who were in Year 8 and from a high-decile school. It would also
seem that Pacific students attending low-decile schools were least
likely to use text structures which ended with a Consequence. This
is in contrast to students who only provided a Premise. These results
are shown in Table 5.12. When these results are compared with those
of students who used a Conclusion, it can be seen quite clearly that
students from high decile schools were more likely to provide their
responses in complex text structures. |
|
|
|
Table
5.12. Use of only Premises in the text. |
|
|
Premises |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
factual
premise |
6 |
3 |
8 |
1 |
5 |
4 |
0 |
9 |
personal
premise |
3 |
8 |
10 |
1 |
7 |
2 |
2 |
11 |
mathematical
premise |
8 |
7 |
2 |
13 |
8 |
4 |
3 |
15 |
|
|
|
Of
the 24 Pacific students, twenty used only Premises. Of these, twelve
were either factual, repeating information in the question, or personal.
Personal Premises were related to an action that the speaker did
which was not mathematical, for example 'I don't know I just guessed'.
Personal Premises, because of their nature, did not occur with a
Consequence and/or Conclusion. It would seem that more Pacific students
were likely to feel that admitting a lack of knowledge was an acceptable
way to respond to a mathematical assessment task. However, it was
also the case that 8 Pacific students recognised that mathematical
calculation was required and were able to provide an appropriate
one. For example a Year 8 girl said, 'Nine minutes times ninety-eight
cars'. Yet, knowing this did not mean that they could actually provide
an appropriate numerical amount, as many found doing this calculation
too hard to do mentally and had no other way of arriving at an approximation.
4 Pacific Year 8 students did the calculation and gave an accurate
response by just supplying a Premise. However, if only a mathematical
Premise was given then it was unlikely that the student would be
able to solve the problem appropriately. The number of Pacific students
who only used a Premise was double the number of students from low
decile schools in areas with a low Pacific population.
In considering
how the different elements were connected, there were not the number
of Premises beginning with 'if … ' as there had been in the Better
Buy task. This can be seen in Table 5.13. |
|
|
|
Table
5.13. Use of logical connectives in the text structure. |
|
|
Logical
Connectives |
Gender |
Year
Level |
School
Decile and Ethnicity |
Total
|
Girls |
Boys |
Year
4 |
Year
8 |
Low
PI |
Low
Non- PI |
High |
 |
'if'
before premise |
5 |
3 |
7 |
1 |
2 |
2 |
4 |
8/72 |
'so'
before a consequence |
4 |
6 |
5 |
5 |
0 |
5 |
5 |
10/29 |
'so'
before a conclusion |
2 |
1 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3/17 |
'and
then' before a consequence |
1 |
7 |
2 |
6 |
0 |
5 |
3 |
8/29 |
'and
then' before conclusion |
0 |
1 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
1/17 |
'and'
before conclusion |
1 |
5 |
3 |
3 |
0 |
3 |
3 |
6/17 |
|
|
|
When
looking at this table it is worth remembering that from the total
of 72 students, only half used a text structure which included more
elements than just the Premise. The total numbers of students who
used each of the elements is given as the second number in the totals
column. The difference in use of logical connectives by gender is
interesting. 18 boys compared to 19 girls used text structures which
consisted of more than just a Premise element. Yet, on the whole,
boys used more logical connectives than girls. |
|
|
|