4.1 |
INTRODUCTION |
The
analysis of Year 4 and 8 students’ responses indicated that fractional
understanding is a complex domain—understanding builds incrementally,
over considerable time, as opposed to being an all-or-nothing occurrence.
Many children in the NEMP study appeared quite confident with solving
fraction problems with reference to the context; however, several of those
who used the formal mathematics of classroom, rather than their informal
knowledge, appeared confused by fraction rules and procedures.
The research reviewed in the introduction (Section 1) indicated that unlike
children’s construction of early number knowledge that often occurs
informally, a great deal of children’s fraction learning take place
through instruction. Teaching fraction operations involves stretching
and transforming the domain of numbers with new ideas such as the incommensurability
of different units for addition, or that multiplication makes smaller
answers rather than larger. Unfortunately, the research also suggests
that much of the current instruction is relatively ineffective in terms
of student learning. As recently as 2002, initial assessments of New Zealand
Year 7-10 students’ in the NEST numeracy project found students
in these year groups to be unexpectedly weak in knowledge of fractions:
“Many teachers
were concerned at the number of their students (10% of all students)
who could not identify unit fractions such as 1/4 or 1/3. A further
40% of year 9 students…could identify unit fractions but could
not successfully complete stage 6 tasks, which involve ordering unit
fractions…” (Irwin & Niederer, p. 47)
To suggest a single
model or recipe for successfully teaching fractions would be naïve.
Susan Lamon, a researcher who has invested considerable energies into
investigating the teaching and learning of fractions, states that although
“we have a much better understanding of the role the presenting
and representing fractions in instruction plays in enabling or disabling
the development of rational-number understanding” we do not have
definitive answers on such issues as “which interpretations can
or should be integrated in instruction” (Lamon, 2001, p. 163). In
response to criticism (e.g., Behr, Wachsmuth, Post, & Lesh, 1984)
that teaching practices have been dominated by experiences of part-whole
representations, with only passing attention to alternative subconstructs,
Lamon contends that teachers must move froward from the part/whole constraint,
which retains a focus on the relative amount, to more frequent use of
part/whole interpretation with unitising
[Unitising refers to the cognitive process of reconceptualizing a quantity
according to some unit of measure that is convenient for thinking about
and operating on, the quantity. For example, the given unit in a fraction
problem may be 12 cans of drink, but a student may think about that quantity
as 1 (12-pack), 2 (6-packs), 3 (4 packs) etc. (see Lamon, 1999 for more
instructional examples)] and the measure interpretation:
Both rely heavily
on the principles of measurement—the inverse relationship between
the size of the unit with which one is measuring and the number of times
one can measure it, out of a given quantity, and the successive partitioning
of that unit of measure into finer and finer subunits until one can
name the amount in a given quantity. (p. 163)
Lamon’s research
suggests that the unitising and measure interpretations and their corresponding
representations assist children to make the transition from whole numbers
to rational numbers because these interpretations build on and extend
principles of measurement with which the children have been familiar since
early childhood. Other researchers (e.g., Empson, 2002; Mack, 1993) also
propose that partitioning and equal sharing are effective starting points
as they provide explicit links with a range of children’s informal
knowledge situations.
Use
of informal knowledge
While
“instruction to assist students in extending and building on their
informal knowledge of rational numbers cannot be prescribed precisely”
(Mack, 1993, p. 95) it is clear that the range of suggested approaches
all utilise young children’s capabilities for mathematical thinking
and knowledge construction (Carpenter et al., 1993). Engaging children
in purposeful, meaningful, and inquiry-based instruction that integrates
their informal knowledge with the instructed knowledge can capitalise
on the need for fractions and the development of actions sequences about
fractions that arise quite early in children’s everyday social activities
(Kieren, 1988; Perry & Dockett, 2002; Smith, 2002). To achieve the
ultimate goal of fraction instruction, that is, that students understand
fractions as numbers in their own right, there are some agreed fundamental
activities that can serve children’s initial reorganisation of fraction
conceptions: equidivision of wholes into parts (sharing), recursive partitioning
of parts (splitting), and reconstruction of the unit (i.e., the whole)
(Pitkethly & Hunting, 1996). For example, using scarce or desirable
objects like biscuits or lollies within contexts involving division of
an object or group of objects amongst friends provides the basis for invented
strategies for equal-sharing problems (Sharp, Garofalo, & Adams, 2002).
In order to acknowledge the initial resources for thinking about fractions
that children bring to the classroom, teachers need to select a representational
domain which is familiar and understandable to the childrenæin other
words, a domain in which they can extend and develop their understanding
of the ideas, as well as their capacity to reason with and about those
ideas (Ball, 1993; Carpenter et al., 1993; Mack, 1990, 1998). The teacher’s
role is to select and construct appropriate models, examples, stories,
illustrations and problems. In the NEMP study the problems involved a
pizza context: many students used the pieces of pizza to explain their
thinking, and in some cases the pieces of pizza were needed to support
the children’s thinking. The Year 4 students, in particular, tended
to largely rely on their informal knowledge of whole number counting rather
than fraction schema to interpret the problems. For example, in response
to the question “How much of the pizza is left?” the link
between 3 pieces of pizza and _ of a pizza was tentative, and needed prompting
by the interviewer.
As children learn, they develop more intuitive knowledge in which they
combine thought, informal language, and images; they become more able
to extract mentally, and think about, fractional ideas without a strong
dependence on the specific context. Those students who were readily able
to switch back and forth between reasoning with whole numbers and naming
the fractional number indicated that their fractional and whole number
sequences had achieved similar levels of abstraction (Olive, 2002).
One of the critical actions in building rational knowledge is the matching
of problems presented in the context of familiar situations to symbolic
representations. Mack (1993) suggests that in order to promote connections
between the symbolic representation and informal knowledge teachers need
to make frequent adjustment of problems to make those that draw on informal
knowledge and those that are presented symbolically more similar. While
there is no doubt that, eventually, children should be able to express
their mathematical ideas using the standard mathematical symbols it is
unnecessary, and even counterproductive, to expect sophisticated symbol
use among young children — who often have developed their own system
of symbols and can use this consistently — until another more standardized
system can be taken on board (Hughes, 1986). To facilitate meaning for
symbols and formal mathematical language connections must be made between
students’ conceptual understanding and informal language —
it is important that the meanings make sense in an interconnected manner
and originate in children’s thinking about things they understand
(Hiebert et al., 1997). Empson (2001) for example, notes that moving children
from solving 24 children sharing 8 pancakes to 8/24 = 1/3 takes time,
and is assisted if concepts such as equivalence, multiplication and division
are allowed to develop concurrently.
The NEMP study findings showed that some students had developed separate
systems of arithmetic that operated independently of one another—an
informal system that they used to solve problems that are meaningful to
them and a school arithmetic system that consisted of procedures that
they applied to the apparently artificial story problem presented to them.
These systems were especially evident in relation to Questions 5 and 6
when answered by Year 8 students using an algorithmic procedure that did
not relate to their verbal explanation/demonstration of the successive
partitioning of pizza pieces. However, within a classroom situation, a
teacher can use these instances as teachable moments to assist students
to recognise the connection between the symbolic representation and the
student’s informal knowledge. For example, in Question 5 and 6 the
student’s meaningful strategy of sharing can be used to provide
an initial context for solving the symbolic problem of 1/2 x 11/4
or 11/4 ÷ 2 as well as providing
a context for discussing equivalent fraction concepts. Building on the
match between students’ reasoning of the problem presented in the
context of the familiar situation will help students to connect their
informal knowledge for rational numbers with the corresponding symbols
and procedures.
Influence
of Context and Representations
When assisting students to link their informal knowledge with symbolic
representations the influence of context is significant. For example,
a commonly reported example of how real-world concerns can collide with
the mathematical viability of a representation concerns the problem of
whether 3/3 is greater than, less than, or the same amount as 5/5? With
reference to a ‘cookie’ context, some children argue that
3/3 is more because each piece (one third) is bigger. Others argue that
5/5 is more because there are more pieces. Still others argue that they
are the same because each represents one whole brownie. However, rather
than be concerned about the influence of problem contexts, teachers can
and should take advantage of the fact that problem contexts can suggest
representations for students to reason with directly — especially
in the initial stages of developing fraction understanding.
The analysis of responses in this study clearly illustrates the influence
of problem context on the students’ reasoning and eventual solution.
As a representational context the pizza problem provided a ‘thinking
space’ for students. Year 4 students, in particular, frequently
solved the problems by referring to the constraints, real or imagined,
of the context (e.g., sharing both types of pizza (ham and pepperoni),
rather than all of the pizza), and provided solutions in the format of
the context (e.g., number of pieces of pizza) rather than the fractional
format. For the majority of students the sharing activity, invoked by
informal partitioning strategies, was an integral part of the solution.
Moreover, even when Year 8 students appeared to use a more formal mathematical
procedure to solve the problem (in only a few cases in Question 5), they
more often resorted to an informal ‘sharing type’ explanation
(Question 6) that involved distributing the available pizza pieces. These
findings parallel those of other researchers investigating children’s
fraction problem solving involving real contexts (Kieren, 1988; Mack,
1990) and children’s problem solving in everyday settings (Carraher
et al. 1987; Lave & Wenger, 1991).
Sharing problems (e.g., sharing 2 biscuits between 2 people, and then
between 3 people, etc.) have been found to provide useful contexts for
which children can effectively build their own models and representations
of real-world phenomena as well as of mathematical ideas (Empson, 2002;
Streefland, 1993). Some examples of sharing problems are found in the
Teaching Fractions, Decimals, and Percentage Numeracy booklet (Ministry
of Education, 2003a). Equal sharing provides a productive context for
several reasons: Firstly, equal sharing relates well to children’s
informal knowledge of partitioning strategies (Carpenter et al., 1993),
and can be used in both continuous and discrete contexts. Secondly, the
used of equal-sharing contexts facilitates varied mathematical interpretations
by children, a key to cognitive change in joint activity (Streefland,
1993). Initially, the most salient aspect of sharing involves the action
of partitioning; the size of the resulting pieces and the relationship
between the number of sharers and number of shared units are of secondary
importance. However, extending the problems to involve ‘fair or
equal’ shares encourages the necessity of parts of equal size; same
number, and exhaustion of the whole. Rational number subconstructs that
are exposed in equal-sharing situations include partitioning, part-whole,
and ratio. Part-whole concepts arise in the context of partitioning and
discussing the resulting pieces and their size relative to a discrete
unit (e.g., dividing each pizza into fourths and assigning 3 of them to
obtain a value of 3/4 of a pizza). Ratio-related constructs result from
conceptualising the number of people sharing and number of items shared
as a composite unit (e.g., recognising that when dividing 3 pizzas among
4 people, each will receive 3/4 of a pizza).
Whatever context that is chosen, selected problems need to be sufficiently
open to afford students opportunities to explore and make conjectures
and follow important mathematical tangents; encouraging learners to confront
and grapple with the conceptual complexity that is inherent in learning
fractions. Additionally, the student needs to be familiar, and preferably
interested, with the selected context — that is, the context needs
to be experientially real to the learner who can then draw on his or her
informal knowledge to develop an effective strategy to solve the problem
(Gravemeijer, 1997b). As noted in Section 1.3 not all contexts hold equal
appeal for all students. As with Mack’s (1993) finding that some
children preferred ice-cream contexts to pizza, it was evident that some
children in this study were ‘interested’ in the context and
models provided, and several took the opportunity to comment on their
pizza flavour preference!
One warning in relation to the use of contexts that teachers need to be
aware of is that in our efforts to link problems to everyday experience
we sometimes create contextual problems that do not in fact ‘match’
reality in a practical sense. For example, consider the following Fraction
question suggested in the NumPA diagnostic test (Ministry of Education,
2002, p. 18):
Q.35 Show the student
the fraction card 8/6. Pizza Picasso cuts their pizzas into sixths.
You buy eight sixths (pointing to 8/6). How much pizza would you
get?
From our analysis
of the NEMP task it is likely that many students in the middle years of
schooling would reference their informal contextual knowledge. However,
in this instance, there is a possibility for confusion: it is very unusual
to buy (or even ask) for eight sixths. If buying by the piece, which would
not normally be the experience of most students, you would buy 8 pieces.
The analysis of the NEMP task suggests that many students who are at the
stage of referencing their informal knowledge would answer “eight
sixths or 8 pieces” and require a lot of prompting to provide the
information that is being sought in this question (designed to assess
students’ ability to coordinate numerators and denominators and
explain the meaning of improper fractions).
Teachers and students often represent realistic contexts as concrete or
pictorial representations, for example circles for pizzas, counters or
Unifix cubes for biscuits etc. These physical representations serve as
useful tools for mathematical communication, thought, and calculation,
allowing personal mathematical ideas to be externalised, shared, and preserved.
In order to help clarify ideas in ways that support reasoning and build
understanding any representation should highlight the conceptual dimensions
of the content, not just its surface or procedural characteristics (Meyer
et al., 2001). As was evidenced in this study the students appeared to
be familiar with the exemplifying objects and were able to use familiar
language to describe and communicate relations among the objects (Ball,
1993). However, the familiarity also created questions related to the
social and cultural appropriateness (e.g., sharing meals in a family situation)
for all children.
Representations are metaphorical, borrowing meaning from one dominant
context to clarify or illuminate something in another, and thus, no one
representational context is perfect. While not definitive in which representations
should be used, in which order, researchers unanimously urge teachers
to pose tasks that will elicit a variety of strategies and representations
involving multiple interpretations of fraction symbols and concepts. Learning
a “new concept is the product of a cross-breeding between several
metaphors rather than a single metaphor” (Sfard, 1997). For example,
equivalence, a source of difficulty for many children, can be explored
in many different ways: e.g., equivalence in area, use of paper folding,
multiplication by identity, decimal representations, are some. Similarly,
a standard region model [see Fraction Exemplar example] is a typical representation
of 1/3 — however, students should also examine mixtures of discrete
and continuous representations involving more than 3 equal pieces, as
well as ratio representations.
|
|
|
(a) A discrete representation, (b) part-whole representation, and
(c) a discrete representation
|
Helping children to
recognise the multiple personality of a rational number is a challenge,
but their ability to detect nuances in a range of problems and to build
meaning when they have no rules or algorithms are strengths that instruction
might exploit.
Within the classroom, representational contexts are co-constructed and
developed by members of the class. Typically, students enter the representational
context that the teacher has established and, in dealing with a specific
problem, they generate alternative ways to represent or check their understandings.
However, when using manipulative materials, like Fraction Tiles and fraction
strips we should be cautious when linking children’s representations
with pre-partitioned materials to their ability to construct and reason
about their own work (Smith, 2002). Students’ work with manipulative
materials does not automatically generate mathematical knowledge (Ball,
1993; Stacey, Helme, Archer, & Condon, 2002). Fraction manipulative
materials skilfully used by teachers can support children’s developing
knowledge of fractions, but only when these materials are seen as representations
of numerous examples of divided quantities experienced previously by children.
In order to reach beyond and across specific situations to abstract and
generalise emergent understandings students must be supported to communicate,
negotiate and reappraise their own understandings in response to the mathematical
norms and values of the classroom (Anthony & Walshaw, 2002).
|